Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:06:00.247Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lexical acquisition and acquisition of initial voiceless stops

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Ann A. Tyler*
Affiliation:
University of Nevada
Mary Louise Edwards
Affiliation:
Syracuse University
*
[*] Address for correspondence: School of Medicine, Department of Speech Pathology & Audiology, Nell J. Redfield Building, 152, Reno, NV 89557-0046, USA.

Abstract

The interaction between lexical acquisition and acquisition of initial voiceless stops was studied in two normally developing children, aged 1;9 and 1;10, by acoustically examining the token-by-token accuracy of initial voiceless stop targets in different lexical items. Production accuracy was also examined as it related to the frequency of usage of different words, as well as the time when they entered the children's lexicons. Fewer than half of the words in the children's lexicons had tokens representing the emergence of accurate voiceless stop production prior to the session at which the voicing contrast was achieved. These words were primarily ‘old’ words that had been in the children's lexicons from the beginning of data collection, as opposed to ‘new’ words, first produced in later recording sessions. Findings are discussed in reference to the ‘lexical diffusion’ model of sound change and within the framework of nonlinear underspecification theory.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Archangeli, D. (1988). Aspects of underspecification theory. Phonology Yearbook 5, 183207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton, D. & Macken, M. A. (1980). An instrumental analysis of the voicing contrast in word-initial stops in the speech of four-year-old English-speaking children. Language and Speech 23, 159–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, M. & Wang, W. S.-Y. (1975). Sound change: actuation and implementation. Language 51, 255–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinnsen, D. (1992). Variation in developing and fully developed phonetic inventories. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological Development. Parkton, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A., Chin, B., Elbert, M. & Powell, T. W. (1990). Some constraints on functionally disordered phonologies: phonetic inventories and phonotactics. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 33, 2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eguchi, S. & Hirsch, I. J. (1969). Development of speech sounds in children. Acta Otolaryngologica Supplementum 257, 151.Google ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. A. (1978). Learning to pronounce: the earliest stages of phonological development in the child. In Minifie, F. D. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds), Communicative and cognitive abilities – early behavioral assessment. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language 51, 419–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, J. H. V. (1977). A voice onset time analysis of apical stop production in 3-year-olds. Journal of Child Language 4, 103–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, M., Barrett, M., Jones, D. & Brookes, S. (1988). Linguistic input and early word meaning. Journal of Child Language 15, 7794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsieh, Hsin-L. (1972). Lexical diffusion: evidence from child language acquisition. Glossa 6, 89104.Google Scholar
Janson, T. (1983). Sound change in perception and production. Language 59, 1834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatt, D. H. (1975). Voice onset time, frication and aspiration in word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 18, 686706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labov, W. (1981). Resolving the Neogrammarian controversy. Language 57, 255–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lisker, L. & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word 20, 384422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, M. & Lieberman, P. (1985). Acoustic analysis of words produced by a child from 46 to 149 weeks. Journal of Child Language 12, 527–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macken, M. A. & Barton, D. (1980). A longitudinal study of the aquisition of the voicing contrast in American English word-initial stops, as measured by voice-onset time. Journal of Child Language 7, 4174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metz, D. E., Conture, E. G. & Caruso, A. (1979). Voice onset time, frication, and aspiration during stutterers' fluent speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 22, 649–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moskowitz, A. I. (1972). Idomatic phonology and phonological change. Unpublished manuscript. University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Sagey, E. (1986). The representation of features and relations in non-linear phonology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Schwartz, R. & Leonard, L. (1982). Do children pick and choose: an examination of phonological selection and avoidance in early lexical acquisition. Journal of Child Language 9, 319–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smit, A. B. & Bernthal, J. E. (1983). Voicing contrasts and their phonological implications in the speech of articulation-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 26, 486500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A. & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Spencer, A. (1986). Towards a theory of phonological development. Lingua 68, 338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stampe, D. (1969). The acquisition of phonetic representation. Paper presented at the 5th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. & Cooper, J. A. (1984). Patterns of early lexical and phonological development. Journal of Child Language 11, 247–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, A. A. (1988). Acoustic analysis of voicing contrast acquisition in normal and phonologically disordered children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY.Google Scholar
Tyler, A. A. & Saxman, J. H. (1991). Initial voicing contrast acquisition in normal and phonologically disordered children. Applied Psycholinguistics 12, 453–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, W. S.-Y. (1969). Competing changes as a cause of residue. Language 45, 925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zebrowski, P. M., Conture, E. G. & Cudahy, E. A. (1985). Acoustic analysis of young stutterers' fluency: preliminary observations. Journal of Fluency Disorders 10, 173–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zlatin, M. A. (1975). Voicing contrast: perceptual and productive voice onset time characteristics of adults. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 56, 981–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zlatin, M. A. & Koenigsknecht, R. A. (1976). Development of the voicing contrast: a comparison of voice onset time in stop perception and production. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 19, 93111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar