Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:27:57.676Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning non-adjacent regularities at age 0 ; 71

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 August 2012

JUDIT GERVAIN*
Affiliation:
CNRS and Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France
JANET F. WERKER
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
*
Address for correspondence: Judit Gervain, Universite Paris Descartes –45 rue des Saints-Peres, Paris 75006, France. e-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

One important mechanism suggested to underlie the acquisition of grammar is rule learning. Indeed, infants aged 0 ; 7 are able to learn rules based on simple identity relations (adjacent repetitions, ABB: “wo fe fe” and non-adjacent repetitions, ABA: “wo fe wo”, respectively; Marcus et al., 1999). One unexplored issue is whether young infants are able to process both adjacent and non-adjacent repetitions. As the previous studies always compared the two types of repetition structures directly, the ability to learn only one of them was sufficient for successful discrimination in these tasks. The present study reports two experiments, in which we test the ability of infants aged 0 ; 7 to discriminate adjacent and non-adjacent repetition structures against random controls (ABB vs. ABC and ABA vs. ABC). We show that, contrary to some previous proposals, infants aged 0 ; 7 successfully discriminate both repetition types from random controls, but show no spontaneous preference for either of them.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[1]

The work was funded by the ANR Jeunes Chercheurs et Jeunes Chercheuses Grant nr. 21373 and a Fyssen Foundation Start-Up grant to JG and NSERC 81103 to JW.

References

REFERENCES

Dutoit, T. (1997). An introduction to text-to-speech synthesis,Vol. 3. Dordrecht, Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Endress, A. D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G. & Mehler, J. (2007). Perceptual constraints and the learnability of simple grammars. Cognition 105, 577614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endress, A. D., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. (2009). Perceptual and memory constraints on language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13, 348–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, Michael C., Slemmer, Jonathan A., Marcus, Gary F. & Johnson, S. P. (2009). Information from multiple modalities helps five-month-olds learn abstract rules. Developmental Science 12, 504509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Pena, M. & Mehler, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105, 14222–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervain, J., Mehler, J., Werker, J. F., Nelson, C. A., Csibra, G., Lloyd-Fox, S., Shukla, M. et al. (2011). Near-infrared spectroscopy: A report from the McDonnell infant methodology consortium. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1, 2246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giurfa, M., Zhang, S., Jenett, A., Menzel, R., & Srinivasan, M. V. (2001). The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect. Nature 410, 930–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gómez, R. L. & Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition 70, 109135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gómez, R. L. & Maye, J. (2005). The developmental trajectory of nonadjacent dependency learning. Infancy 7, 183206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guasti, M. T. (2002). Language acquisition: The growth of grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. P, Fernandes, K. J., Frank, M. C, Kirkham, N., Marcus, G., Rabagliati, H. & Slemmer, J. A. (2009). Abstract rule learning for visual sequences in 8- and 11-month-olds. Infancy 14, 218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lany, J. & Gómez, R. L. (2008). Twelve-month-old infants benefit from prior experience in statistical learning 19, 1247–52.Google ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G. F, Fernandes, K. J. & Johnson, S. P. (2007). Infant rule learning facilitated by speech. Psychological Science 18, 387–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Rao, S. B. & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283, 7780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, J. R., Pollak, S. D., Seibel, R. L. & Shkolnik, A. (2007). Dog is a dog is a dog: Infant rule learning is not specific to language. Cognition 105, 669–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santelmann, L. M. & Jusczyk, P. W. (1998). Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: Evidence for limitations in processing space. Cognition 69, 105134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Heugten, M. & Johnson, E. K. (2010). Linking infants' distributional learning abilities to natural language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language 63, 197209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, J. B., Fox, S. E., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Nelson, Charles A. (2011). Neural processing of repetition and non-repetition grammars in 7- and 9-month-old infants. Frontiers in Psychology 2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed