Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T05:55:31.200Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning discourse topic management in the preschool years*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Susan H. Foster
Affiliation:
Northern Arizona University

Abstract

The ability of children aged 0;1 to 2;6 to initiate and maintain topics of conversation is explored using videotaped data of mother–child interaction collected at home. An analysis of both verbal and non-verbal behaviours suggests that initially children attract attention exclusively to themselves as topics of conversation. The emergence of manipulative skills from 0; 5 and the development of deictic gestures from around the end of the first year facilitate the initiation of topics concerned with items in the physical environment. Finally, with the development of language, reference to intangibles becomes possible. In maintaining topics, children are initially capable of extended sequences only within the context of routines, but by 2;6 are able to maintain coherent topics independently. The implications of the analysis presented for understanding the role of prelinguistic communication in language development are discussed against the background of a modular framework.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1986

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Andersen, E. & Kekelis, L. (1983). The role of children in determining the nature of their linguistic input. Paper presented at the Eighth Annual Conference on Language Development,Boston University.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: the acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (with the collaboration of L. Benigni, I. Bretherton, L. Camaioni & V. Volterra) (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & MacWhinney, B. (1979). A functionalist approach to the acquisition of grammar. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (eds), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, L. (1981). Language as a product of dialogue. Discourse Processes 4. 117–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult–child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. CogPsych 8. 521–52.Google Scholar
Bridges, A. (1979). Directing two-year-olds' attention; some clues to understanding. JChLang 6. 211–26.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1974 a). The ontogenesis of speech acts. JChLang 2. 119.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1974 b). The organization of early skilled action. In Richards, M. P. M. (ed.), The integration of a child into a social world. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1974/1975). From communication to language: a psychological perspective. Cognition 3. 255–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1983). The acquisition of pragmatic commitments. In Golinkoff, R. (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carter, A. L. (1975). The transformation of sensorimotor morphemes into words: a case study of the development of ‘more’ and ‘mine’. JChLang 2. 233–50.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1978). From gesture to word: on the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In Bruner, J. S. & Garton, A. (eds), Human growth and development: Wolfson College lectures 1976. Oxford: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Clark, R. (1974). Performing without competence. JChLang 1. 110.Google Scholar
Fine, J. (1978). Conversation, cohesive and thematic patterning in children's dialogues. Discourse Processes 1. 247–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, S. H. (1979). Topic initiation – one step or two: some factors involved in mother–child interaction at the prelinguistic stage. Nottingham Linguistic Circular 8. 96107.Google Scholar
Foster, S. H. (1981 a). Interpreting child discourse. In French, P. & MacLure, M. (eds), Adult–child conversation. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Foster, S. H. (1981 b). The emergence of topic type in children under 2; 6: a chicken-and-egg problem. PRCLD 20. 5260.Google Scholar
Foster, S. H. (1982). Learning to develop a topic. PRCLD 21. 6370.Google Scholar
Foster, S. H. (1984). Modularity and language acquisition: distinguishing syntax and pragmatics. Paper presented to the International Association for the Study of Child Language,Austin, Texas.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1974). Some properties of social play. MPQ 20. 163–80.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. & Gordon, L. (1983). In the beginning was the word: a history of the study of language acquisition. In Golinkoff, R. M. (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1980). Toward an operational and logical analysis of intentionality: the use of discourse in early child language. In Olson, D. (ed.), The social foundations of language and thought. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Howe, C. (1981). Acquiring language in a conversational context. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. & Charney, R. (1981). Conversational asymmetry between mothers and children. JChLang 8. 3549.Google ScholarPubMed
Keenan, E. & Klein, E. (1975). Coherency in children's discourse. JPsycholingRes 4. 365–80.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. & Schieffelin, B. (1976). Topic as a discourse notion: a study of topic in the conversation of children and adults. In Li, C. (ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Newhoff, M. & Fey, M. E. (1980). Some instances of word usage in the absence of comprehension. JChLang 7. 189–96.Google Scholar
Luszcz, M. A. & Bacharach, V. R. (1983). The emergence of communicative competence: detection of conversational topics. JChLang 10. 623–37.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1972). The cognitive basis of language learning in infants. PsychRev 79. 113.Google ScholarPubMed
Masur, E. F. (1982). Mothers' responses to infants' object-related gestures: influences on lexical development. JChLang 9. 2330.Google ScholarPubMed
McDonald, L. & Pien, D. (1982). Mother conversational behaviour as a function of interactional intent. JChLang 9. 337–58.Google ScholarPubMed
Michaels, S. & Cook-Gumperz, J. (1979). A study of sharing time with first-grade students: discourse narratives in the classroom. Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistic Society 5. 647–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, C. M. (1978). Pointing in the context of shared activity. ChDev 49. 371–80.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. JChLang 5. 115.Google Scholar
Peters, A. (1977). Language learning strategies: does the whole equal the sum of the parts? Lg 53. 560–73.Google Scholar
Ratner, N. & Bruner, J. (1978). Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language. JChLang 5. 391401.Google ScholarPubMed
Romanes, G. (1888). Mental evolution in man: origin of human faculty. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, J. (1974). Early language development: towards a communicational analysis. In Richards, M. P. M. (ed.), The integration of a child into a social world. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R., Hepburn, A. & Collis, G. M. (1983). Verbal and non-verbal aspects of mothers' directives. JChLang 10. 337–55.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up closings. Semiotica 8. 289327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnur, E. & Shatz, M. (1984). The role of maternal gesturing in conversations with one-year-olds. JChLang 11. 2941.Google ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. JChLang 4. 122.Google Scholar
Snow, C. & Goldfield, B. (1983). Turn the page please: situation-specific language acquisition. JChLang 10. 551–69.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
van Dijk, T. A. (1980). Macrostructures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Watson-Gegeo, K. A. & Boggs, S. T. (1977). From verbal play to talk story: the role of routines in speech events among Hawaiian children. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Kernan, C. Mitchell (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weir, R. H. (1962). Language in the crib. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar