Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T07:27:17.553Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Interest, home environment, and young Chinese children’s development of English as a second/foreign language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2024

Xinyi Zhang*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Education, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Carrie Lau
Affiliation:
Faculty of Education, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
*
Corresponding author: Xinyi Zhang; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The current study examined the relationship between interest, the home environment, and young Chinese children’s development of English as a second/foreign language in Hong Kong. Two hundred and seventy-four Hong Kong kindergartners were assessed on their interest in learning English and their English language skills (i.e., expressive and receptive vocabulary). Their parents completed questionnaires eliciting family socio-economic status, parental beliefs, and home learning environment. The results indicated that (1) interest was related to children’s English language abilities after controlling for children’s gender, non-verbal intelligence, and kindergarten type; (2) parents’ beliefs about their child’s English ability and self-efficacy were related to children’s interest in learning English; and (3) interest uniquely contributed to children’s English language ability in the home environment. The present findings provide evidence of the active role that children play in their second/foreign language development and highlight the significant influence of parental beliefs on children’s interest in learning English.

摘要

摘要

本研究以香港爲背景 , 探討興趣、家庭環境與幼兒英語作爲第二語或外語發展之間的關繫 。研究測量了274名香港幼稚園學童對學習英語的興趣及其英語语言能力 (即表達性及接收性詞匯) 。幼兒的家長填寫了有關家庭社經地位、家長信念和家庭學習環境的問卷 。研究結果顯示 : (1) 在控製了幼兒的性別、非語言智力和幼稚園類型等條件後 , 學習興趣與幼兒的英語语言能力相關 ; (2) 家長對其孩子英語能力的信念和自我效能感與幼兒學習英語的興趣相關 ; (3) 學習興趣在家庭環境中對幼兒的英語语言能力有獨特的貢獻 。本研究的結果表明幼兒的學習興趣在其第二語或外語發展中發揮著積極作用 , 並強調家長信念對幼兒的英語學習興趣有著關鍵的影響。

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Early language development lays the foundation for later language and literacy achievement (Scarborough et al., Reference Scarborough, Neuman, Dickinson, Fletcher-Campbell, Soler and Reid2009; Whitehurst & Lonigan, Reference Whitehurst and Lonigan1998). The development of language proficiency from as early as 2–4 years old acts as a precursor for the acquisition of preliteracy skills and linguistic comprehension, which forms a crucial basis for later literacy development (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2002; Dixon, Chuang et al., Reference Dixon, Chuang and Quiroz2012; Oakhill & Cain, Reference Oakhill and Cain2012; Scarborough, Reference Scarborough1990; Scarborough & Dobrich, Reference Scarborough and Dobrich1994; Storch & Whitehurst, Reference Storch and Whitehurst2002). According to the lexical restructuring hypothesis (Walley et al., Reference Walley, Metsala and Garlock2003), vocabulary – a critical language skill – is a prerequisite skill for the development of phonological awareness. It has been found that environmental influences can impact the development of phonological awareness only when young children’s vocabulary reached a certain threshold (Dixon, Chuang et al., Reference Dixon, Chuang and Quiroz2012). Therefore, it is necessary to understand the potential factors that may influence language acquisition in the early years (Goodrich et al., Reference Goodrich, Lonigan, Phillips, Farver and Wilson2021).

Language development occurs within social contexts (Clark, Reference Clark2017). Before formal schooling, the home environment is one of the most critical language-learning contexts for young children (Frijters et al., Reference Frijters, Barron and Brunello2000; Sénéchal & LeFevre, Reference Sénéchal and LeFevre2014; Tse et al., Reference Tse, Zhu, Hui and Ng2017). However, children are not passively shaped by their environment, and children’s characteristics can interact with their surrounding environment to influence developmental outcomes (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerne2006). Interest, for example, has long been identified as a strong predictor of children’s learning and development (Hidi, Reference Hidi1990; Schiefele, Reference Schiefele1991). More specifically, there is evidence showing that interest is linked to young children’s language acquisition and further, functions as an underlying mechanism in the relationship between the home environment and children’s language development (Farver et al., Reference Farver, Xu, Eppe and Lonigan2006; Malin et al., Reference Malin, Cabrera and Rowe2014).

However, previous research on interest and early language development has mostly been concerned with first language (e.g., Lukie et al., Reference Lukie, Skwarchuk, LeFevre and Sowinski2014; Malin et al., Reference Malin, Cabrera and Rowe2014). There is a paucity of studies investigating the extent to which interest contributes to young children’s second/foreign language (S/FL) development, especially in Asian contexts (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Lau and Su2021). Considering the prevalence of English as a second/foreign language (ESL/EFL) learning in the pre-school period in Asia and the importance of English language proficiency in academic achievement and career opportunities (Butler, Reference Butler2015; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Kang, Cho and Sheo2019; Kirkpatrick & Liddicoat, Reference Kirkpatrick, Liddicoat, Bolton, Botha and Kirkpatrick2020), this is an important research gap, which needs to be addressed. Further, even less is known about the influence of interest in S/FL learning in the home environment on early S/FL development. As children’s characteristics can interact with home environmental factors in shaping their own development (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerne2006), interest needs to be examined within the home environment when investigating its influence on a child’s development. Therefore, the present study aims to examine (1) the extent to which interest contributes to young Chinese children’s language development in ESL/EFL; (2) how the home environment relates to young Chinese children’s interest in ESL/EFL learning; and (3) the potential effect of the interaction between the home environment and interest on young Chinese children’s language development in ESL/EFL.

Interest

Interest is generally defined as a personal preference for particular objects or activities (Renninger, Reference Renninger, Sansone and Harackiewicz2000; Schiefele, Reference Schiefele1991). Researchers have differentiated two types of interest: situational interest and individual interest (Hidi, Reference Hidi1990; Schiefele, Reference Schiefele1991). Situational interest refers to a positive response triggered by certain environmental stimuli, which is often transient and thus cannot promote substantial learning or development (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Alexander, Spencer, Leibham and Neitzel2004). In contrast, individual interest is an ongoing and stable personal predilection for a specific subject, which is characterised by positive attitudes, full engagement, predictable task orientation, and an increasingly enriched knowledge base (Renninger, Reference Renninger, Sansone and Harackiewicz2000). Individual interest has been found to account for significant variances in children’s achievement (e.g., Hidi & Renninger, Reference Hidi and Renninger2006).

Schiefele (Reference Schiefele1991) identified two components of interest: a feeling-based component and a value-based component. The feeling-based component is mainly concerned with positive feelings or affect, which reflect the extent to which a child may find joy in a certain activity or event. The value-based component refers to the perceived importance attached to particular objects. Given that individual interest has more significant and long-term effects on children’s development than situational interest (Hidi & Renninger, Reference Hidi and Renninger2006; Renninger, Reference Renninger, Sansone and Harackiewicz2000), and that our participants – three- to four-year-old children – may not be able to build the relevant knowledge and recognise the personal significance of the target object that forms the basis of value-based interest (Schiefele, Reference Schiefele1991), the operational definition of interest employed in the current study mainly focuses on individual interest and the feeling-based component.

Children can exhibit interest in particular domains as early as within the first year of life (DeLoache et al., Reference DeLoache, Simcock and Macari2007), and such early interest is a significant mental resource for children’s learning and development (Hidi, Reference Hidi1990; Schiefele, Reference Schiefele1991). Interest can motivate children to engage in learning activities, enhance and maintain children’s attention, improve children’s memory, and influence various aspects of information processing (Renninger, Reference Renninger, Sansone and Harackiewicz2000). Existing studies have established the association between young children’s interest and early literacy development (Baroody & Diamond, Reference Baroody and Diamond2012; Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Holliman, Weir and Baroody2019; Scarborough & Dobrich, Reference Scarborough and Dobrich1994). The evidence specifically related to language development tends to be less, though there are preliminary findings showing that young children’s interest in reading as early as 27 months could predict their later expressive language development (Deckner et al., Reference Deckner, Adamson and Bakeman2006).

Interest, as a product of the interaction between the child self and the environment, can be stimulated and sustained through both internal and external factors (DeLoache et al., Reference DeLoache, Simcock and Macari2007; Hidi & Renninger, Reference Hidi and Renninger2006; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Alexander, Spencer, Leibham and Neitzel2004). According to the four-phase model of interest development (Hidi & Renninger, Reference Hidi and Renninger2006), the development of interest is a cumulative process originating in transitory situational interests and, if maintained, develops into a stable and enduring predisposition. During the process, the home environment, as the most immediate learning context for young children, can be particularly important in terms of its role in cultivating and deepening the development of children’s interest (Hume et al., Reference Hume, Lonigan and McQueen2015; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Alexander, Spencer, Leibham and Neitzel2004). Various factors within the home environment are found to influence children’s learning interest. For instance, research indicates that children from an enriched home learning environment (HLE), with various home literacy activities and developmentally appropriate learning materials, tend to show higher levels of literacy interest (Baker & Scher, Reference Baker and Scher2002; Hume et al., Reference Hume, Lonigan and McQueen2015; Lonigan, Reference Lonigan1994). In addition, children whose parents value the importance of home learning opportunities and hold positive beliefs about literacy activities and their children’s abilities are more likely to have a strong interest in language and literacy learning (DeBaryshe, Reference DeBaryshe1995; Weigel et al., Reference Weigel, Martin and Bennett2006).

Although existing studies primarily investigated the influences of the home environment on young children’s literacy interest or children’s interest in their first language learning in Western contexts (e.g., DeBaryshe, Reference DeBaryshe1995; Frijters et al., Reference Frijters, Barron and Brunello2000; Hume et al., Reference Hume, Lonigan and McQueen2015; Weigel et al., Reference Weigel, Martin and Bennett2006), Choi and her colleagues conducted a series of studies on young Korean children’s interests in EFL learning which indicated that parental beliefs and motivation, as well as home English learning experiences such as parent–child English interaction and provision of home learning activities and materials were related to young children’s interest in English learning (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Kang, Cho and Sheo2019; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Kim, Kiaer and Morgan-Brown2020; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Kang and Kim2022). However, these findings should be considered as preliminary in Asian contexts given that the studies were all conducted in Korea and focused only on specific aspects of parental beliefs (i.e., maternal motivation for providing English education) and HLE (i.e., home English interactions). As interest is a content-specific concept (Hidi & Renninger, Reference Hidi and Renninger2006), the evidence built on young children’s literacy interest or interest in first language learning cannot be directly generalised to young children’s interest in S/FL. The relationship between the home environment and interest in learning ESL/EFL, especially in Asian contexts, needs to be further explored.

Home environment and young children’s language development

Various aspects of the home environment have been found to exert significant and long-term influences on young children’s language development (Attig & Weinert, Reference Attig and Weinert2020; Lopez, Reference Lopez2021; Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Riggs, Guyon-Harris, Harrison and Huth-Bocks2019; San San Kyaw et al., Reference San San Kyaw, Tin Tin, Underwood and Grant2019). According to bioecological theory (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerne2006), the home environment functions as a complex system within which both proximal and distal processes exist to influence children’s development. Proximal processes, referring to the interactions between the developing child and the people, objects, or symbols in the child’s immediate environment, are regarded as the primary mechanisms underlying children’s development. The HLE is examined most in the understanding of the relationship between proximal processes and children’s development (e.g., Rodriguez & Tamis‐LeMonda, Reference Rodriguez and Tamis‐LeMonda2011; Son & Morrison, Reference Son and Morrison2010). The HLE is a multifaceted concept with various components (Yeung & King, Reference Yeung and King2016). For example, home learning activities, especially language-related activities (e.g., singing songs, watching educational TV programs, and playing language games) and informal literacy activities (i.e., meaning-oriented literacy experiences), have been widely found to be associated with young children’s language development, both in the first language (e.g., Dickinson et al., Reference Dickinson, Griffith, Golinkoff and Hirsh-Pasek2012; San San Kyaw et al., Reference San San Kyaw, Tin Tin, Underwood and Grant2019; Sénéchal & LeFevre, Reference Sénéchal and LeFevre2002, Reference Sénéchal and LeFevre2014), and in S/FLs (e.g., Chow et al., Reference Chow, McBride-Chang and Cheung2010; Dixon, Wu et al., Reference Dixon, Wu and Daraghmeh2012; Yeung & King, Reference Yeung and King2016).

Home learning resources – the availability of reading materials at home is another unique factor in the HLE to facilitate young children’s language development (Georgiou et al., Reference Georgiou, Inoue and Parrila2021; Goodrich et al., Reference Goodrich, Lonigan, Phillips, Farver and Wilson2021; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Martin, Brooks-Gunn and Petrill2008; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Georgiou and Manolitsis2018; Yeung & King, Reference Yeung and King2016). Compared to parent-led learning activities, home literacy materials may provide children with opportunities to actively engage in language learning experiences (Georgiou et al., Reference Georgiou, Inoue and Parrila2021). Home language use is also important for children’s language learning (Dixon et al., Reference Dixon, Chuang and Quiroz2012). However, mixed results have emerged in investigations into home language use and S/FL development. While studies in Asian contexts have found positive effects of home English use on young children’s language development in ESL/EFL (Dixon, Wu et al., Reference Dixon, Wu and Daraghmeh2012; Dulay et al., Reference Dulay, Tong and McBride2017), such relationships have not been identified among ESL learners in English-dominant contexts (Cha & Goldenberg, Reference Cha and Goldenberg2015; Place & Hoff, Reference Place and Hoff2011). The inconsistent findings may be due to the distinctions between English-dominant contexts and ESL/EFL language contexts in terms of English language input in the social environments (Dulay et al., Reference Dulay, Tong and McBride2017; Håkansson & Håkansson & Norrby, Reference Håkansson and Norrby2010; Oller & Eilers, Reference Oller and Eilers2002). It is noted that although HLE is assumed to influence children’s development directly, there is preliminary evidence showing that children’s learning interest can mediate the association between HLE and early language development (Farver et al., Reference Farver, Xu, Eppe and Lonigan2006). In other words, a rich HLE may stimulate children’s learning interest, and children with higher interest may have better achievements in language acquisition.

Distal processes, on the other hand, refer to factors that tend to affect children’s development indirectly, such as family demographics and parents’ characteristics (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerne2006). In this study, we focused on two distal factors in the home environment – family SES (i.e., Socioeconomic Status) and parental beliefs – which are empirically and theoretically believed to influence children’s language acquisition through their effects on the HLE: families with higher SES backgrounds can provide their children with greater language exposure and more language learning opportunities and resources at home than their lower SES counterparts, and parents who hold positive beliefs about supporting young children’s language development tend to enable a rich HLE, which in turn influences children’s language developmental outcomes (e.g., Hoff, Reference Hoff2013; Hoff & Tian, Reference Hoff and Tian2005; Huttenlocher et al., Reference Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea and Hedges2010; Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Mancilla-Martinez, Flores and McClain2020; Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Lau and Tan2023; Pace et al., Reference Pace, Luo, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2017; Ronderos et al., Reference Ronderos, Castilla-Earls and Marissa Ramos2021). However, there seems to be additional factors that link family SES or parental beliefs to young children’s language development. The review conducted by Pace et al. (Reference Pace, Luo, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2017) indicated that children’s characteristics can be a significant source underlying the influences of family SES on young children’s language development. Considering the positive associations between family SES/parental beliefs and children’s learning interest (Baker & Scher, Reference Baker and Scher2002; Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Yang, Dulay and McBride2018; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Kang, Cho and Sheo2019) and further, between interest and children’s developmental outcomes (Baroody & Diamond, Reference Baroody and Diamond2012; Bracken & Fischel, Reference Bracken and Fischel2008; Deckner et al., Reference Deckner, Adamson and Bakeman2006), it is possible that children’s interest in S/FL learning may be a potential mechanism underlying the relation between distal processes (e.g., family SES and parental beliefs) and language developmental outcomes.

In summary, both proximal and distal factors in the home environment are found to influence young children’s language development. Consistent with the bioecological model (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerne2006), previous findings provide support on the direct correlations between multiple components of the HLE (i.e., proximal factors in the home environment) and early language acquisition, as well as the indirect association between family SES and parental beliefs (i.e., distal factors in the home environment) and young children’s language growth through the HLE. However, it is noted that parents’ learning-support practices and beliefs, as well as their connections with children’s language development, can be shaped by sociocultural contexts (Cha & Goldenberg, Reference Cha and Goldenberg2015; Davis-Kean & Sexton, Reference Davis-Kean and Sexton2009; Dulay et al., Reference Dulay, Tong and McBride2017; Oller & Eilers, Reference Oller and Eilers2002; Riches & Curdt-Christiansen, Reference Riches and Curdt-Christiansen2010). Furthermore, there is evidence showing that the function of proximal factors (i.e., HLE) on early language development may be further mediated by children’s learning interest, which can also act as a source of the underlying mechanism of distal factors (i.e., parental beliefs and family SES) on language developmental outcomes.

The current study

Research indicates that interest can contribute to young children’s learning and development (Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Holliman, Weir and Baroody2019; Deckner et al., Reference Deckner, Adamson and Bakeman2006; Renninger, Reference Renninger, Sansone and Harackiewicz2000). Additionally, it may play a role in the influences of the home environment – one of the most critical early learning contexts–on young children’s language developmental outcomes (Farver et al., Reference Farver, Xu, Eppe and Lonigan2006; Malin et al., Reference Malin, Cabrera and Rowe2014). However, the existing evidence has been mostly built on emergent literacy (e.g., Baroody & Diamond, Reference Baroody and Diamond2012; Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Holliman, Weir and Baroody2019; Scarborough & Dobrich, Reference Scarborough and Dobrich1994), first language development (e.g., Farver et al., Reference Farver, Xu, Eppe and Lonigan2006), and Western contexts (e.g., Malin et al., Reference Malin, Cabrera and Rowe2014). Despite the significance of early language acquisition and the prevalence of ESL/EFL education in Asian contexts, there is a lack of studies investigating the role of interest in young Asian children’s language development in ESL/EFL, especially within the home environment. Considering that the impacts of interest on developmental outcomes can be domain-specific (Deckner et al., Reference Deckner, Adamson and Bakeman2006; Lukie et al., Reference Lukie, Skwarchuk, LeFevre and Sowinski2014) and that sociocultural contexts may influence the associations between home factors and children’s development (Davis-Kean & Sexton, Reference Davis-Kean and Sexton2009; Riches & Curdt-Christiansen, Reference Riches and Curdt-Christiansen2010), this research gap is important and necessary to address. Therefore, this study aims to address the following research questions:

  1. (1) To what extent does young Chinese children’s interest in learning English contribute to their English language development after controlling for children’s gender, non-verbal intelligence, and the type of kindergarten that they attend?

  2. (2) What home environmental factors contribute to young Chinese children’s interest in learning English?

  3. (3) Does young Chinese children’s interest in learning English uniquely contribute to their language development in ESL/EFL after taking into account the home environment? Furthermore, does interest mediate the associations between home environmental factors and young Chinese children’s English language development?

Based on existing literature, we predicted that (1) interest would make a significant contribution to young Chinese children’s language development in ESL/EFL; (2) both proximal processes (i.e., the HLE) and distal processes (i.e., family SES and parental beliefs) would influence children’s interest in learning English; and (3) interest and the home environment would uniquely and jointly influence young children’s English language development. More specifically, both HLE and interest would mediate the relationship between distal processes in the home environment (including parental beliefs and family SES) and children’s English language development, and interest would further mediate the relationship between HLE and young children’s English language development.

The current study was conducted in Hong Kong. Hong Kong is considered to be in both an ESL and EFL context (Li, Reference Li2017). English is one of the official languages in Hong Kong and is used in various social contexts, including education, government, the mass media, and law (Kirkpatrick & Liddicoat, Reference Kirkpatrick, Liddicoat, Bolton, Botha and Kirkpatrick2020). However, Chinese is predominantly used in society, and English to some extent, functions as a foreign language as well (Li, Reference Li2017). Similar to many other Asian countries and societies, English is highly valued in Hong Kong and is introduced in schools from as early as the pre-school years (i.e., 3- to 6-year-old) (Lau & Richards, Reference Lau and Richards2021). Some Hong Kong parents also attach great importance to their children’s English language development and tend to provide their children with various types of English language support, such as private English language tutoring, English-speaking domestic helpers, and home English language resources and activities (Li, Reference Li2017).

We sampled 3- to 4-year-old Chinese children from K1 kindergarten classrooms (i.e., the first year of kindergarten). Language acquisition begins early in life, and the significance of language development on later achievement can be identified between 2 and 4 years old (Scarborough, Reference Scarborough1990; Storch & Whitehurst, Reference Storch and Whitehurst2002). Moreover, an age-related decrease in the ability of acquiring a S/FL has been reported from 4 to 6 years old (Berk, Reference Berk, Campanella, Mosher, Ashkenaz, McLellan, Harris and Pauken2013; Newport et al., Reference Newport, Bavelier, Neville and Dupoux2001) and thus, investigating the environmental influences on S/FL development in early stages (e.g., 3–4 years old) can be important and necessary. Furthermore, it is common for Hong Kong children to receive a certain degree of English exposure both at home and in kindergarten from as early as 3–4 years old (i.e., K1 level) (Forey et al., Reference Forey, Besser and Sampson2016; Li, Reference Li2017). Considering that the current study focuses on the home environment and young Chinese children’s English development, K1 level can be an appropriate period to investigate home influences and minimise the potential confounding effects of school experiences compared to higher kindergarten levels. For English language abilities, we assessed children’s expressive and receptive vocabulary, which are most commonly used as an index of language developmental outcomes among studies investigating ESL/EFL development in Asian contexts (see a review Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Lau and Su2021).

Method

Participants

Two hundred and seventy-four Hong Kong kindergarten children ( $ 126\;\mathrm{girls},{\mathrm{Mean}}_{age}=3;11.11, SD=3.65 $ ) and their parents participated in the study. The participating children were recruited from 28 kindergartens located in three districts of Hong Kong (i.e., 9 in Hong Kong Island, 9 in Kowloon, and 10 in the New Territories). Kindergartens in Hong Kong provide services for 3- to 6-year-old children and can be categorised as being either non-profit-making or private independent depending on the nature of the sponsoring organisations (i.e., non-profit agencies or private enterprises). Kindergartens can also be classified as local or international. Local kindergartens follow the local curriculum and English is taught as a subject, while Chinese is the main language of instruction. International kindergartens generally offer bilingual/trilingual (i.e., Cantonese, Putonghua, and English) programs. Most of the participating kindergartens (N = 24;86%) were local non-profit making kindergartens, while four were private independent and international kindergartens.

The participating children were ethnic Chinese and were in their first year of kindergarten (i.e., K1 class for 3-to 4-year-olds). Most of the children’s parents (71%) had completed secondary school education or held a higher diploma, 28% had completed undergraduate education or above, and less than 1% of the parents had only completed primary school. The family monthly income ranged from less than HK$10,000 per month (4%) to more than HK$100,000 per month (8%). More than one-third of the families earned either HK$10,000–29,999 per month (36%) or HK$30,000–59,999 per month (36%). 11% of the families earned HK$60,000–79,999 per month, and 5% earned HK$80,000–99,999 per month.

Procedure

The data used in the current study were from a larger, longitudinal project on the role of the home and school in Hong Kong children’s English development. Ethical approval was obtained from the Human Research Ethics Committee of the authors’ university before data collection. Consent forms were signed by the participating kindergarten principals, teachers, and parents, and oral assent was obtained from the participating children. Parents completed questionnaires that collected information on family demographics, parents’ beliefs about supporting their young children’s English development, and HLE. The questionnaires were distributed and collected through the kindergarten teachers in sealed envelopes.

Children were assessed on their English language abilities (i.e., English expressive and receptive vocabulary), non-verbal intelligence, and interest in learning English. The assessments were conducted in a quiet place in the kindergartens by trained assessors who had experience in early childhood education. The duration of the assessment for each child was about 20–30 min.

Measures

Family SES background

Family SES background was assessed using two indicators: parents’ educational attainment and household monthly income. Both mother’s and father’s highest educational attainment was measured on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = primary school, 2 = junior high school, 3 = senior high school, 4 = higher certificate, 5 = bachelor’s degree, 6 = master’s degree, and 7 = doctoral degree). Household monthly income was measured on a 10-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = less than HK$4,000 to 10 = more than HK$100,000, with increments of HK$9,999 (except 1 = less than HK$4,000, and 2 = HK$4,000-9,999). An average score was calculated for maternal and paternal educational attainment. Standardised z-scores were computed for parents’ average educational attainment and household monthly income. A composite score of the z-scores was calculated and used as the index of family SES.

Parents’ beliefs about supporting young children’s English development

The Parent English Language Belief and Attitude Questionnaire (PELBA-Q; Zhang & Lau, Reference Zhang and Lau2022) was employed to assess parents’ beliefs about supporting their young child’s English development. The PELBA-Q was developed based on the Theory of Planned Behaviour (Ajzen, Reference Ajzen2012) and aimed at assessing parental beliefs about, and attitudes towards, supporting young children’s ESL/EFL development. The questionnaire consists of four factors: (1) Perceived importance of the home environment (e.g., “I think the home environment is important for my child’s English learning”); (2) Perceived child’s English ability and self-efficacy (e.g., “In general, I think my child is good at English”; “I am very confident that I can support my child in learning English very well”); (3) Normative beliefs (e.g., “When it comes to supporting my child’s English learning, some people’s/books’/organizations’ opinions and suggestions are important to me [e.g., other parents, friends, colleagues, teachers, experts, educational books, school, or government guidelines]”); and (4) Control beliefs (e.g., “I think pre-schoolers are too young to learn English. I would like my child to learn English after he/she enters primary school”).

According to the Theory of Planned Behaviour (Ajzen, Reference Ajzen2012), the first two factors (i.e., perceived importance of the home environment and perceived child’s English ability and self-efficacy) constitute behavioural beliefs, which refer to an individual’s perceptions of the consequences of the target behaviour and their overall feelings towards the perceived consequences. Behavioural beliefs are assumed to exert the most significant influence on the prediction of the target behaviour (Ajzen, Reference Ajzen2012). Normative beliefs refer to the perceived expectations or behaviours of important others, such as the practices of other parents; the suggestions from teachers or schools; and the recommendations from curriculum guidelines, relevant experts or reference books; as well as the willingness to comply with such expectations or practices. Control beliefs refer to the beliefs that may hinder people from engaging in the target behaviour and thus exert a negative effect on the prediction of the behaviours. For the current study, we included three of the factors – perceived importance of the home environment, perceived child’s English ability and self-efficacy, and control beliefs. We did not include the factor of normative beliefs because more than 90% of the participants tend to agree with the items under this factor and the lack of variability can constrain its ability to produce meaningful information for further data analysis. It makes sense given that Hong Kong parents tend to value the importance of young children’s English language acquisition, and kindergartens, education books, or government policies and documents in Hong Kong (potential important referents) generally encourage early ES/FL acquisition (Leung et al., Reference Leung, Lim and Li2013). The items of the questionnaire are rated on a 7-point Likert scale (i.e., from 1=strongly disagree to 7=strongly agree). The Cronbach’s alpha reliability for the three factors ranged from $ .75 $ to $ .89 $ .

HLE

Several aspects of HLE were assessed by the questionnaire developed by Lau and Richards (Reference Lau and Richards2021) in Hong Kong: (1) the frequency of home English learning activities (e.g., reading English language books, English storytelling, playing English games, and singing English songs); (2) the number of English books at home; and (3) home language use. The frequency of engagement in each of the home English learning activities was rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (Never) to 6 (Daily). The number of English books available in the home was coded on a 7-point Likert scale (0 = none, 1 = 1–20, 2 = 21–40, 3 = 41–60, 4 = 61–80, 5 = 81–100, 6 = more than 100). Home language use was coded as either (1) Chinese only or (2) English is (at least) one of the main home languages. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability was $ 0.70 $ for the current sample.

Child assessment

Interest. To assess children’s interest in learning English, an interview was conducted with each child following the procedure suggested by Marsh et al. (Reference Marsh, Craven and Debus1998). During the interview, each child was asked about their feelings towards five common English learning activities (i.e., reading English books, watching English TV programs, listening to English stories or songs, speaking English with other people, and learning English). Children were first asked a binary question (e.g., “Do you like reading English books or not?”) and were expected to give a “yes” or “no” response. Based on the response to the first question, the assessor further asked the child the extent to which he/she liked or disliked the activity. “Dislike it a lot” was coded as 1, “Dislike it a little” as 2, “Like it a little” as 3, and “Like it a lot” as 4. The maximum score was 20. If the child did not respond (i.e., either to the first or the second question), the assessor explained the question further and asked once more. If the child was still unable to respond, the non-response was treated as missing data. The assessment shows acceptable reliability in our sample (Cronbach’s $ \alpha =0.74 $ ).

English Language. Receptive vocabulary. Two subsets of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-IV (PPVT-4, Dunn & Dunn, Reference Dunn and Dunn2007) were employed to assess receptive vocabulary. The assessors presented four pictures to the child each time and pronounced a word. The child was asked to select one of the four pictures that corresponded to the word presented. There were 2 training items and 24 test items. Each correct answer was awarded one point, and the maximum score was 24. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability in our sample was $ 0.86 $ .

Expressive vocabulary. For the assessment of English expressive vocabulary, the Expressive Vocabulary Test-II edition (EVT-2; Williams, Reference Williams2007) was employed. The assessor presented one picture to the child each time and asked a stimulus question (e.g., what do you see?). The child was expected to answer the question using a word corresponding to the picture presented. There were 20 items in total. Each correct response was given one point, and the maximum score was 20. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability was $ 0.94 $ in this sample.

Non-verbal Intelligence. The Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Sets A and B; Raven et al., Reference Raven, Court and Raven1995) were used to assess non-verbal intelligence. The child was presented with a matrix-like pattern, which included one missing part and was asked to choose one piece out of six options to complete the pattern. There were 24 items, and each correct answer was given one point. The maximum score was 24. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability in our sample was $ 0.63 $ .

Statistical analysis

For data screening, we first deleted cases with more than 35% missing values ( $ N=4 $ ). Then, we examined the pattern and distribution of the missing data. Five variables had missing data, and a total of 0.5% of the values were missing. No systematic missing pattern was identified. Based on the results of Little’s MCAR test, the data were missing completely at random (MCAR) ( $ p>.05 $ ). In the following statistical analyses, we employed full information maximum likelihood (FIML; Arbuckle, Reference Arbuckle, Marcoulides and Schumacker1996) to deal with missing data. Afterwards, as latent variables were involved, we conducted descriptive analysis and examined the correlational relationships between the variables in Mplus 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2017). In addition, we conducted confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) for the latent variables (i.e., English language outcomes and parental belief factors).

To examine the hypothesised model of interest, the home environment, and young children’s ESL/EFL development (see Figure 1), we conducted structural equation modelling in Mplus 8.3 with Maximum Likelihood (ML) estimation procedure. In the model, children’s gender, nonverbal intelligence, and kindergarten type were treated as control variables, which are beyond the scope of the current study but can potentially influence children’s English language development, especially in the Hong Kong context (e.g., Eriksson et al., Reference Eriksson, Marschik, Tulviste, Almgren, Pérez Pereira, Wehberg and Gallego2012; Lai-Reeve et al., Reference Lai-Reeve, Tak-Ming Wong and Li2018; van der Schuit et al., Reference van der Schuit, Segers, van Balkom and Verhoeven2011). The model was specified such that parental beliefs (i.e., perceived importance of the home environment, perceived child English ability and self-efficacy, and control beliefs) and family SES served as both direct and indirect predictors of young Chinese children’s interest in learning English and their English language developmental outcomes through HLE (i.e., home English learning activities, home language use, and home English book resources). Further, children’s interest in learning English was treated as a predictor of English language outcomes, as well as a mediator in the relationship between home environmental factors (i.e., HLE, parental beliefs, and family SES) and English language development. We used the standard suggested by Hu and Bentler (Reference Hu and Bentler1999) to evaluate the model fit: comparative fit index(CFI) $ \ge 0.90 $ ; root Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) $ \ge 0.90 $ ; mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) $ \le 0.08 $ ; and standardised root mean square residual (SRMR) $ \le 0.10 $ . Further, we examined the mediation effects of interest or home variables using the bootstrapping technique with 5000 resamples (Preacher & Hayes, Reference Preacher and Hayes2008).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Figure 1. The Model Depicting Relations Between Home Environment, Interest, and Young Chinese Children’s English Language Development.

Notes. *p < .05; **p < .01; Lan = English language outcomes; RV = receptive vocabulary; EV = expressive vocabulary; PIH = Parents’ perceived importance of the home environment; CES = Parents’ perceived child’s English ability and self-efficacy; CB = control beliefs; HEA = Home English activities; HL = home language use; BR = English book resources; SES = family SES; CI = children’s interest in learning English; Children’s gender, non-verbal intelligence, and the type of kindergartens were controlled in the model.

Results

Correlation and regression analysis

The results of the correlational analysis (Table 2) showed that interest was positively related to English language development ( $ r=.25,p<.01 $ ). Four factors in the home environment were related to children’s interest in learning English. Specifically, parents’ perception of their child’s English ability and self-efficacy displayed the strongest correlation with interest ( $ r=.23,\ p<.01 $ ), followed by home English learning activities ( $ r=.18,\ p<.01 $ ). Parental beliefs about the importance of the home environment and home language use were also related to children’s interest in learning English, but the correlational coefficients were relatively small ( $ r=.15,\ p<.05 $ ; $ r=.12,\ p<.05 $ ). English language outcomes were correlated with all the HLE factors (i.e., home English learning activities, home English book resources, and home language use) ( $ rs=.37-.40;\ ps<.01 $ ) and family SES ( $ r=.50;\ p<.01 $ ). Regarding parental beliefs, English language was positively related to parents’ perception of their child’s English ability and self-efficacy ( $ r=.43;p<.01 $ ) and parents’ beliefs about the importance of the home environment ( $ r=.19;p<.01 $ ) but negatively associated with parents’ control beliefs ( $ r=-.29;\ p<.01 $ ).

Table 2. Correlations for the home environment, interest, and English language development

Notes. *p<.05; **p<.01; HL=Home language use; HEA=home English activities; BR=Number of English book resources; PIH=Parents’ perceived importance of the home environment; CES=Parents’ perceived child’s English ability and self-efficacy; CB=Control beliefs

Table 3. Total, direct, and indirect effects of parental beliefs and family SES on English language development

Note:. HEA = Home English activities; HL = Home language; BR = Home English book resources; Lan = English language outcomes; *p < .05; **p < .01

Structural equation modelling analysis

To investigate the relationship between interest, the home environment, and young Chinese children’s English language development, we examined the SEM (i.e., Structural Equation Modeling) model outlined above (see Figure 1). The model fit indices were acceptable ( $ {x}^2=454.4,\ df=258,\ p<.01; RMSEA=.05,90\% CI=\left[.05,.06\right]; CFI=.93; TLI=.91; SRMR=.06 $ ). The values for the endogenous variables were 7% for children’s interest in learning English, 45% for English language outcomes, 18% for home language use, 30% for home English book resources, and 26% for home English learning activities. The results showed that, after controlling for children’s gender and non-verbal intelligence, as well as the type of kindergarten, children’s interest in learning English was significantly related to English language outcomes ( $ \beta =.13,p<.05 $ ). Among all the home environmental factors, only parents’ perceptions of their child’s English ability and self-efficacy were associated with children’s interest in learning English ( $ \beta =.22,p<.05 $ ). The expected indirect paths from parents’ beliefs or family SES to interest through HLE were not significant.

For the relationship between the home environment and English language outcomes, two distal factors (i.e., parents’ perceptions of their child’s English ability and self-efficacy, and family SES) ( $ \beta s=.21-.26,\ p<.01\ or\ p<.05 $ ) and two HLE factors (i.e., home language use and English book resources) ( $ \beta s=.14-.17,\ p<.01\ or\ p<.05 $ ) were correlated with English language developmental outcomes. Further, family SES is indirectly related to English language outcomes through its effects on home language use ( $ {\beta}_{ind}=.03,\ S.E.=.01,\ p<.05;\ 95\%\ CI=\left[.01,.06\right];7\%\ of\ the\ total\ effects $ ) and English book resources ( $ {\beta}_{ind}=.07,\ S.E.=.03,\ p<.05;\ 95\% CI=\left[.02,.13\right];\ 15\% of\ the\ total\ effects $ ). Similarly, parents’ perception of their child’s English ability and self-efficacy was indirectly associated with English language outcomes through home language use ( $ {\beta}_{ind}=.06,\ S.E.=.02,\ p<.01;\ 95\% CI=\left[.03,.10\right];17\%\ of\ the\ total\ effects $ ). However, the direct effects of family SES and parental beliefs on English language outcomes accounted for the majority of the total effects (i.e., 69% and 62%, respectively). The indirect effects of home variables on English language development through children’s interest in learning English were not identified.

Discussion

The present study investigated the relationship between interest, the home environment, and young Chinese children’s language development in ESL/EFL. The results indicated that (1) interest contributed to the young Chinese children’s English language development after controlling for children’s gender, non-verbal intelligence, and the type of kindergarten; (2) among the various home factors, only parents’ perceptions of their children’s English ability and self-efficacy were related to children’s interest in learning English; and (3) in the home environment, interest uniquely contributed to English language outcomes. The findings are broadly aligned with previous research and theoretical assumptions, which highlight children’s active role in their development (e.g., Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerne2006; Scarborough & Dobrich, Reference Scarborough and Dobrich1994; Whitehurst & Lonigan, Reference Whitehurst and Lonigan1998). However, some of our findings are inconsistent with the existing literature, especially concerning the interaction between the home environment and interest. This study may shed light on the complexity of the home environment in terms of its contribution to S/FL development.

Interest and language development in ESL/EFL

The present study indicated that, after controlling for children’s gender, non-verbal intelligence, and kindergarten type, interest was significantly related to young Chinese children’s English language outcomes. This finding seems to be contrary to many of the previous studies which show that interest was mostly associated with young children’s literacy outcomes but was not or only modestly related to language development (e.g., Baroody & Diamond, Reference Baroody and Diamond2012; Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Holliman, Weir and Baroody2019; Deckner et al., Reference Deckner, Adamson and Bakeman2006; Frijters et al., Reference Frijters, Barron and Brunello2000). The mixed results may stem from the different measurements of interest. Previous research tended to focus exclusively on literacy interest and assessed young children’s interest by asking them about their feelings or rating their affective responses towards literacy/print-focused activities (e.g., reading books, visiting the library, learning about the names of letters) (e.g., Baroody & Diamond, Reference Baroody and Diamond2012; Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Holliman, Weir and Baroody2019; Deckner et al., Reference Deckner, Adamson and Bakeman2006; Frijters et al., Reference Frijters, Barron and Brunello2000). In contrast, the current study was based on the most common English learning activities for 3–4-year-old Chinese children in Hong Kong, which includes both literacy/print-focused activities (e.g., reading English books and learning English words/letters) and activities aimed at facilitating oral language development (e.g., speaking English with others; listening to English songs; and watching English TV programs).

This seems to imply that interest, as a content-specific concept, can contribute to specific domains of children’s language and literacy development (Deckner et al., Reference Deckner, Adamson and Bakeman2006; Lukie et al., Reference Lukie, Skwarchuk, LeFevre and Sowinski2014). In addition to the relationship between interest and literacy development in early childhood, the present study provides support to the contribution of interest on young children’s language development. Further, the differing findings between previous studies and the current study may suggest that future research should adopt more fine-grained measurements of interest to investigate the role of interest in distinct areas of early language and literacy development.

Furthermore, as the present study focused on feeling-based interest, which reflects the level of children’s enjoyment in English learning activities, the current findings highlight the importance of positive feelings in the process of learning English for early ESL/EFL acquisition. In other words, young children who enjoy and feel happy during English learning activities tend to have better language development outcomes in ESL/EFL. It has long been established that repeated practice and use of English facilitates oral language development (Cazden, Reference Cazden1972; Snyder-McLean & McLean, Reference Snyder-McLean and McLean1978), this finding may come from the fact that young children with higher interest in learning English would be more willing to use English, practice English, and be engaged in English learning activities. This study also provides support to educational beliefs or tenets about young children learning through play – emphasising the significance of affective experiences in learning activities (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Kang, Cho and Sheo2019; Putman & Walker, Reference Putman and Walker2010). Considering studies suggesting Asian children’s gradual decrease in motivation and increased dissatisfaction in learning ESL/EFL (Butler, Reference Butler2015), the current results may shed light on the importance of stimulating children’s interest in the S/FL learning process. However, future research is necessary to further confirm the underlying mechanism of interest in young children’s ESL/EFL development.

Home environment and interest

Among the various factors in the home environment, parents’ perceptions of their child’s English ability and self-efficacy were the only factors that significantly contributed to children’s interest in learning English. This suggests that young children whose parents hold positive beliefs about their child’s English ability and feel confident in supporting their child’s English development have more interest in learning English. The importance of parental beliefs in promoting children’s intrinsic learning interest in early childhood period seems to exceed that of providing material resources and learning opportunities at home. Our findings align with previous research demonstrating that parents’ positive beliefs make unique contributions to children’s interest and achievement (Baker & Scher, Reference Baker and Scher2002). Parents’ positive beliefs about their children’s ability may promote the children’s self-confidence (Frome & Eccles, Reference Frome and Eccles1998), which forms an important basis for children’s learning interest (Aunola et al., Reference Aunola, Nurmi, Niemi, Lerkkanen and Rasku‐Puttonen2002). Additionally, parents’ beliefs may convey directly or indirectly through certain inter-generational mechanisms to their children, which influence children’s interests and feelings (Baker & Scher, Reference Baker and Scher2002).

Although previous studies have found that learning activities and resources provided by parents can stimulate and maintain children’s interest in specific learning domains (Hume et al., Reference Hume, Lonigan and McQueen2015; Lonigan, Reference Lonigan1994), the findings of the present study did not identify any HLE factors related to young Chinese children’s interest in learning English. One potential reason for the mixed findings may be related to how interest is measured. A number of previous studies used the frequency of engagement in learning activities as an indicator of children’s interest, based on the assumption that children with higher interest would engage more frequently in related activities (e.g., Hume et al., Reference Hume, Lonigan and McQueen2015; Lukie et al., Reference Lukie, Skwarchuk, LeFevre and Sowinski2014). However, such measurements may share conceptual overlaps with HLE itself, which is generally measured by including the frequency of engagement in various learning activities, and thus result in high correlations between interest and the HLE. Another reason may be related to the age group of the children under study. Young children and beginning learners naturally hold more positive perspectives on language and literacy learning compared to older learners (Baker & Scher, Reference Baker and Scher2002). Consistent with previous research (e.g., Baker & Scher, Reference Baker and Scher2002; Baroody & Diamond, Reference Baroody and Diamond2012; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Kang, Cho and Sheo2019), the present study showed that the participating children, on average, exhibit high interest in learning English (as indicated by the high mean score and low standard deviations in the measurement of interest). Thus, it is possible that children’s natural characteristics during this age period, rather than the external environment, constitute a major factor in influencing children’s learning interest.

Furthermore, considering that the existing evidence on the HLE and interest is mainly obtained in Western contexts (e.g., Hume et al., Reference Hume, Lonigan and McQueen2015; Lonigan, Reference Lonigan1994; Lukie et al., Reference Lukie, Skwarchuk, LeFevre and Sowinski2014), it is possible that sociocultural factors also play a role in the relationship between the HLE and children’s learning interest. Sociocultural contexts within which the families are located can largely shape how parents create and organise the HLE and the approach or methods they use when conducting learning activities (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Anderson, Friedrich and Kim2010; Hoff & Tian, Reference Hoff and Tian2005; Janes & Kermani, Reference Janes and Kermani2001), which in turn can impact the relationship between the HLE and children’s developmental outcomes (Sun et al., Reference Sun, Yin, Amsah and O’brien2018; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Lau and Su2021). Thus, future studies may consider making cultural comparisons to confirm the role of sociocultural factors in the associations between the HLE and young children’s learning interest in ESL/EFL and further investigate the underlying influencing mechanisms. Moreover, it is important for future research to look beyond the categorical and quantitative aspect of home learning experiences, which cannot reflect how exactly the learning activities are conducted and how the learning resources are arranged and may obscure cultural and social differences in terms of their influences on young children’s learning interest or motivation (Baker & Scher, Reference Baker and Scher2002). Indeed, Malin et al. (Reference Malin, Cabrera and Rowe2014) found that it was parents’ reading quality, rather than reading frequency, that predicted children’s reading interest. It is reasonable to assume that the process of the activities, especially affective-related experiences, determines the extent to which children enjoy the learning activities, which in turn largely affects children’s interest. The insignificant results in the current study may also have resulted from our focus solely on the quantity, and not the quality, of home English learning activities.

Although family SES background has been found to influence various domains of children’s development (e.g., Liu et al., Reference Liu, Georgiou and Manolitsis2018; Tse et al., Reference Tse, Zhu, Hui and Ng2017), we found no significant relationships between family SES and young children’s interest in learning English. Young children from lower SES family backgrounds did not necessarily have less interest in learning English. Considering that interest can relate to language outcomes in ESL/EFL, this finding might imply that interest can act as a buffering factor for children from low-family SES backgrounds in ESL/EFL learning. Further, this finding may inform the design of interventions that enable parents, especially those from low SES backgrounds, to provide positive affective experiences in home English learning activities and cultivate children’s interest in learning English so as to promote early ESL/EFL development.

Interest, home environment, and young Chinese children’s language development in ESL/EFL

Both interest and home environmental factors made significant and unique contributions to young Chinese children’s English language outcomes after controlling for children’s gender, non-verbal intelligence, and the type of kindergarten. Surprisingly, the distal factors in the home environment (i.e., parental beliefs and family SES), rather than the proximal factors, were found to be the strongest predictors of English language outcomes. Consistent with the theoretical assumptions of the bioecological model (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerne2006), family SES and parental beliefs, as distal factors in the home environment, exert indirect influences on children’s English language developmental outcomes through the proximal factors (i.e., the HLE). Families with higher SES backgrounds and parents who are more confident about their child’s English learning abilities and their own abilities to support their child’s English development tend to use more English language at home and provide more English book resources, which directly benefit young children’s language development in ESL/EFL. This finding echoes previous research on young children’s first language development or conducted in societies where the second language is the dominant social language (DeBaryshe, Reference DeBaryshe1995; Hoff, Reference Hoff2013; Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Mancilla-Martinez, Flores and McClain2020).

However, in addition to their indirect influences through the HLE, the direct effects of parental beliefs and family SES on English language development accounted for more than half of the total effects. There may be other factors apart from the HLE that underlie the relationship between family SES, parental beliefs, and young children’s ESL/EFL outcomes. For example, Asian parents with higher SES and who hold stronger beliefs about supporting their child’s English development are more likely to set aside significant resources for English learning including private English tutoring, extracurricular classes in English, and English language programs or activities (Butler, Reference Butler2015; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Kang, Cho and Sheo2019). It is possible that the influencing mechanism of family SES backgrounds and parental beliefs on young children’s ESL/EFL development goes beyond the provision of a rich home English learning environment, especially in ESL/EFL contexts. Future research may extend the scope of the current study and further explore the influencing mechanism of distal factors in the home environment (e.g., family SES and parental beliefs), other than the HLE, on young children’s language development in S/FL learning. Such potential studies could have important implications for promoting parents’ positive roles in stimulating young children’s English language development in ESL/EFL contexts.

For the proximal factors in the home environment, consistent with Dixon et al. (Reference Dixon, Chuang and Quiroz2012)‘s finding in Singapore, English-language use at home turned out to be the most significant factor related to young Chinese children’s English language development. This finding may provide further support for the assumption that although using English at home may not be important for ESL children living in English-speaking societies (Cha & Goldenberg, Reference Cha and Goldenberg2015; Place & Hoff, Reference Place and Hoff2011), it can be beneficial to the development of English language acquisition in ESL/EFL contexts. In addition, English book resources, rather than English learning activities at home, were found to be related to young children’s English language outcomes. This is consistent with the findings obtained from both monolingual children and bilingual children, which indicated that access to learning resources makes unique contributions to children’s language development (Georgiou et al., Reference Georgiou, Inoue and Parrila2021; Yeung & King, Reference Yeung and King2016). Considering that factors such as parents’ English proficiency levels, comfort level in using English, skills and knowledge of supporting children’s English learning, and availability can all influence the extent to which English learning activities are provided at home (Dixon, Wu, et al., Reference Dixon, Chuang and Quiroz2012; Lau & Richards, Reference Lau and Richards2021), this finding may suggest that providing children with English learning materials can be an effective way to facilitate children’s ESL/EFL learning. This implication may shed light on guiding parents’ practices in supporting their children’s English development at home, especially for families who are not able to organise rich home English learning activities. However, this finding should be considered preliminary given that we used only the number of English books as the index of home English learning resources. Future research should confirm these results by taking into account different English learning resources (e.g., electronic resources for English learning) and investigating the underlying mechanisms of the functions of home learning materials on young children’s S/FL development.

Although the contributions of interest to young Chinese children’s English language development were identified in the model, the expected interaction effects between the home environment and interest were not found. Interest and the home environment seem to be independent predictors of young children’s language development (Carroll et al., Reference Carroll, Holliman, Weir and Baroody2019; Frijters et al., Reference Frijters, Barron and Brunello2000). However, the insignificant interaction effects can be related to the participants’ age (three- to four-year-old children naturally hold positive views on language learning) and the fact that the present study only focused on the quantitative aspects of the HLE. Malin et al. (Reference Malin, Cabrera and Rowe2014) found that children’s reading interest mediated the relationship between the quality of parent−child reading (e.g., parents’ metalingual talk), but not the quantitative aspect (i.e., frequency), and young children’s vocabulary development. Therefore, the insignificant findings do not necessarily mean that the interaction between interest and the HLE does not exist in young children’s S/FL development. Future research should consider comparing different age groups and further investigating the potential interaction between interest and other aspects of the home environment (e.g., parent−child interactions during English-learning activities) on young children’s S/FL development.

Limitations and future directions

There are several limitations of the current study. First, the present study employed a cross-sectional design rather than a longitudinal design. Therefore, the relationships between interest, home environment, and English developmental outcomes identified in our study should be considered as correlational in nature, and causal relationships cannot be assumed. The present study found that parents’ perceptions of their children’s English ability and self-efficacy predicted the young Chinese children’s interest in learning English and their English language development. However, this finding can also be explained by children’s English language performance, which can affect parents’ beliefs about their child’s English ability. It is possible that parents feel more confident about their children’s English ability if their children have higher levels of English language achievement and show more interest in learning English. Thus, future longitudinal research that examines the causal relationship between home environmental factors and interest is necessary. Second, the present study only assessed young children’s feeling-based interest, which may not reflect the multifaceted nature of the concept of interest (Schiefele, Reference Schiefele1991). As discussed previously, interest is a content-specific concept (Schiefele, Reference Schiefele1991), and different components of children’s interest can potentially be related to distinct domains of children’s development. Future research could employ a more comprehensive measure of interest. Further, we assessed interest exclusively by child report interview method, which can be vulnerable to social desirability (Bergen & Labonté, Reference Bergen and Labonté2020). Future research may consider using multiple reporting sources (e.g., parents’ questionnaires and observations) to obtain more subjective assessment results on young children’s interest in learning English.

Third, the current study only considered the influence of quantitative HLE factors (i.e., frequency) on young Chinese children’s interest and ESL/EFL development. It has been found that the quantity and quality of the home learning environment exert different influences on young children’s learning interest, as well as on their language and literacy outcomes (Deckner et al., Reference Deckner, Adamson and Bakeman2006; Malin et al., Reference Malin, Cabrera and Rowe2014). Therefore, it is necessary for future research to investigate the potential interaction between the qualitative aspect of the home environment and children’s interest in early S/FL acquisition. Finally, the present study only sampled first-year kindergarten children in the Hong Kong context. Given the influence of sociocultural contexts on parents’ beliefs and practices, and the distinct features of different developmental periods (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2002; Riches & Curdt-Christiansen, Reference Riches and Curdt-Christiansen2010), it remains unknown whether the current findings can be generalized to other sociocultural contexts in Asia, and whether the influence of the home environment on children’s interest in learning English, as well as the role of children’s interest in S/FL development, changes with age. Future cross-cultural or longitudinal studies are needed to confirm the current findings in other Asian contexts and among different age groups.

Implications

The findings of the current study suggest that children play an active role in their own S/FL development. Specifically, the study found that young Chinese children’s interest in learning English was related to their English language developmental outcomes. Given the prevalence of English language education for young children in the Asian context (Butler, Reference Butler2015), the current findings highlight the importance of providing children with positive affective experiences in S/FL learning activities so as to cultivate their learning interest, which may in turn benefit their ESL/EFL achievement. In addition, the study found that parents’ perceptions of young Chinese children’s English ability and self-efficacy uniquely contributed to the children’s interest in learning English and significantly predicted the children’s English language outcomes. Thus, raising parents’ awareness of the importance of developing positive views of their child’s abilities, as well as their own abilities to support their child’s English development, may be an effective way to enable parents to stimulate their children’s interest in learning English and promote the children’s English learning. The findings can also inform educational programs aimed at promoting young children’s S/FL development through collaboration with parents. Furthermore, the study found that family SES was not a significant predictor of interest, though it was one of the strongest predictors of young Chinese children’s English language outcomes. On the one hand, it may suggest that it is important for policy makers or educators to take actions to minimise the disparities between SES groups in early S/FL acquisition. On the other hands, the current findings imply that children’s interest can possibly act as a buffering factor in the S/FL development of children from low SES families, and thus, interventions aimed at supporting young children from low SES backgrounds should consider cultivating children’s interest in learning English.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by Research Grants Council, University Grants Committee that was granted to the second author. Grant # 17619319.

Competing interest

The author(s) declare none.

Footnotes

This manuscript is based on a PhD thesis being completed by the first author under the supervision of the second author.

References

Aunola, K., Nurmi, J. E., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M. K., & Rasku‐Puttonen, H. (2002). Developmental dynamics of achievement strategies, reading performance, and parental beliefs. Reading Research Quarterly, 37(3), 310327. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.37.3.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajzen, I. (2012). Martin Fishbein’s legacy: The reasoned action approach. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 640, 1127. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716211423363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J., Anderson, A., Friedrich, N., & Kim, J. E. (2010). Taking stock of family literacy: Some contemporary perspectives. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10(1), 3353. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798409357387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbuckle, J.L. (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In Marcoulides, G.A. & Schumacker, R.E. (Eds.). Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp.243277). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Attig, M., & Weinert, S. (2020). What impacts early language skills? Effects of social disparities and different process characteristics of the home learning environment in the first 2 years. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.557751CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, L., & Scher, D. (2002). Beginning readers’ motivation for reading in relation to parental beliefs and home reading experiences. Reading Psychology, 23(4), 239269. https://doi.org/10.1080/713775283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baroody, A. E., & Diamond, K. E. (2012). Links among home literacy environment, literacy interest, and emergent literacy skills in preschoolers at risk for reading difficulties. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 32(2), 7887. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271121410392803CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergen, N., & Labonté, R. (2020). “Everything is perfect, and we have no problems”: Detecting and limiting social desirability bias in qualitative researchQualitative Health Research30(5), 783792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berk, L. E. (2013). Language development. In Campanella, C., Mosher, J., Ashkenaz, J., McLellan, L., Harris, S., & Pauken, T. (Eds.), Child development (pp.358399). Pearson.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2002). Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language Learning, 52(1), 159199. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9922.00180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracken, S. S., & Fischel, J. E. (2008). Family reading behavior and early literacy skills in preschool children from low-income backgroundsEarly Education and Development19(1), 4567. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280701838835CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Damon, W., & Lerne, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 793828). Wiley: Theoretical Models of Human Development.Google Scholar
Butler, Y. G. (2015). English language education among young learners in East Asia: A review of current research (2004–2014). Language Teaching48(3), 303342. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0261444815000105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. M., Holliman, A. J., Weir, F., and Baroody, A. E. (2019). Literacy interest, home literacy environment and emergent literacy skills in preschoolers. Journal of Research in Reading, 42(1), 150161. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cazden, C. B. (1972). Child language and education. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Cha, K., & Goldenberg, C. (2015). The complex relationship between bilingual home language input and kindergarten children’s Spanish and English oral proficiencies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(4), 935953. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, S. K., Yang, X., Dulay, K. M., & McBride, C. (2018). Family and individual variables associated with young Filipino children’s numeracy interest and competenceBritish Journal of Developmental Psychology36(2), 334 353. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, N., Kang, S., Cho, H. J., & Sheo, J. (2019). Promoting young children’s interest in learning English in EFL context: The role of mothers. Education Sciences, 9(1), 46. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci9010046CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, N., Kang, S., & Kim, D. (2022). Effects of temperament and parent–child interactions on children’s interest in learning English: The case of english immersion institutionsThe Asia-Pacific Education Researcher31(6), 647656. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40299-021-00615-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, N., Kim, T., Kiaer, J., & Morgan-Brown, J. (2020). Mothers’ educational beliefs and preschoolers’ English learning attitudes: The mediating role of English experiences at homeSAGE Open10(4), 2158244020970231. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244020970231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chow, B. W. Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Cheung, H. (2010). Parent-child reading in English as a second language: Effects on language and literacy development of Chinese kindergarteners. Journal of Research in Reading, 33(3), 284301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01414.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2017). Conversation and language acquisition: A pragmatic approach. Language Learning and Development, 14 ( 3), 170 185.doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2017.13408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis-Kean, P. E., & Sexton, H. R. (2009). Race differences in parental influences on child achievement: Multiple pathways to success. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 55(3), 285318. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.0.0023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeBaryshe, B. D. (1995). Maternal belief systems: Linchpin in the home reading process. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16(1), 120. https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(95)90013-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deckner, D. F., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2006). Child and maternal contributions to shared reading: Effects on language and literacy development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 3141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2005.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLoache, J. S., Simcock, G., & Macari, S. (2007). Planes, trains, automobiles--and tea sets: Extremely intense interests in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 15791586. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1579CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D. K., Griffith, J. A., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2012). How reading books fosters language development around the worldChild Development Research2012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, L. Q., Chuang, H. K., & Quiroz, B. (2012). English phonological awareness in bilinguals: a cross‐linguistic study of Tamil, Malay and Chinese English‐language learners. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(4), 372392. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01471.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, L. Q., Wu, S., & Daraghmeh, A. (2012). Profiles in bilingualism: Factors influencing kindergartners’ language proficiency. Early Childhood Education Journal, 40(1), 2534. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-011-0491-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dulay, K. M., Tong, X., & McBride, C. (2017). The role of foreign domestic helpers in Hong Kong Chinese children’s English and Chinese skills: A longitudinal study. Language Learning, 67(2), 321347. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). PPVT-4: Peabody picture vocabulary test. Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., … & Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communitiesBritish Journal of Developmental Psychology30(2), 326343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farver, J. A. M., Xu, Y., Eppe, S., & Lonigan, C. J. (2006). Home environments and young Latino children’s school readiness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21(2), 196212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.04.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forey, G., Besser, S., & Sampson, N. (2016). Parental involvement in foreign language learning: The case of Hong Kong. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 16(3), 383413. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798415597469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijters, J. C., Barron, R. W., and Brunello, M. (2000). Direct and mediated influences of home literacy and literacy interest on pre-readers’ oral vocabulary and early written language skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 466477. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frome, P. M., & Eccles, J. S. (1998). Parents’ influence on children’s achievement-related perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 435452. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.435CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodrich, J. M., Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Farver, J. M., & Wilson, K. D. (2021). Influences of the home language and literacy environment on Spanish and English vocabulary growth among dual language learnersEarly Childhood Research Quarterly57, 2739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgiou, G. K., Inoue, T., & Parrila, R. (2021). Developmental relations between home literacy environment, reading interest, and reading skills: evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 116. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev. 13589.Google ScholarPubMed
Håkansson, G., & Norrby, C. (2010). Environmental influence on language acquisition: Comparing second and foreign language acquisition of Swedish. Language Learning, 60(3), 628650. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00569. xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental resource for learning. Review of Educational Research, 60(4), 549571. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543060004549CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 111127. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 414https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38(4), 271278https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural equation modeling: a multidisciplinary journal, 6(1), 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, L. E., Lonigan, C. J., & McQueen, J. D. (2015). Children’s literacy interest and its relation to parents’ literacy‐promoting practices. Journal of Research in Reading, 38(2), 172193. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01548.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343365https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hwang, J. K., Mancilla-Martinez, J., Flores, I., & McClain, J. B. (2020). The relationship among home language use, parental beliefs, and Spanish-speaking children’s vocabulary. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 119https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2020.1747Google ScholarPubMed
Janes, H., & Kermani, H. (2001). Caregivers’ story reading to young children in family literacy programs: Pleasure or punishment? Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 44(5), 458466.Google Scholar
Jiang, Y., Lau, C., & Tan, C. Y. (2023). Socioeconomic status and children’s English language and literacy outcomes: The mediating role of home literacy environment. Early Education and Development, 127.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. E., Alexander, J. M., Spencer, S., Leibham, M. E., & Neitzel, C. (2004). Factors associated with the early emergence of intense interests within conceptual domains. Cognitive Development, 19(3), 325343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, A. D., Martin, A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Petrill, S. A. (2008). Order in the house! Associations among household chaos, the home literacy environment, maternal reading ability, and children’s early reading. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (Wayne State University. Press), 54(4), 445472. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.0.0009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkpatrick, A., & Liddicoat, A. J. (2020). English and language policies in east and southeast Asia. In Bolton, KingsleyBotha, Werner, & Kirkpatrick, Andy (Eds.). The handbook of Asian Englishes (pp.81105). Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai-Reeve, S., Tak-Ming Wong, B., & Li, K. C. (2018). Hong Kong teachers’ English oral input in kindergarten classroomsInternational Online Journal of Education and Teaching5(4), 687703.Google Scholar
Lau, C., & Richards, B. (2021). Home literacy environment and children’s English language and literacy skills in Hong Kong. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 569581. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.569581CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, C. S. S., Lim, S. E. A., & Li, Y. L. (2013). Implementation of the HongKong language policy in pre-school settings. Early Child Developmentand Care, 183(10), 13811396. http://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2013.788816CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, D. C. (2017). Multilingual Hong Kong: Languages, literacies, and identities. Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2019.1571070CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, C., Georgiou, G. K., & Manolitsis, G. (2018). Modeling the relationships of parents’ expectations, family’s SES, and home literacy environment with emergent literacy skills and word reading in Chinese. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 43, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonigan, C. J. (1994). Reading to preschoolers exposed: Is the emperor really naked? Developmental Review, 14(3), 303323. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1994.1011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, J. T. (2021). The role of early parenting practices, home environment, and children’s regulation in predicting language development in emerging bilingual children (Doctoral dissertation). Arizona State University.Google Scholar
Lukie, I. K., Skwarchuk, S. L., LeFevre, J. A., & Sowinski, C. (2014). The role of child interests and collaborative parent–child interactions in fostering numeracy and literacy development in Canadian homes. Early Childhood Education Journal, 42(4), 251259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-013-0604-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malin, J. L., Cabrera, N. J., & Rowe, M. L. (2014). Low-income minority mothers’ and fathers’ reading and children’s interest: Longitudinal contributions to children’s receptive vocabulary skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 425432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. W., Craven, R., & Debus, R. (1998). Structure, stability, and development of young children’s self‐concepts: A multicohort–multioccasion study. Child Development, 69(4), 10301053. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06159.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., Bavelier, D., & Neville, H. J. (2001). Critical thinking about critical periods: Perspectives on a critical period for language acquisition. In Dupoux, E. (Ed.), Language, brain and cognitive development:Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp.481502). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(2), 91121. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2010.529219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.). (2002). Language and literacy in bilingual children. Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language developmentAnnual Review of Linguistics3, 285308. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-034226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. C., Riggs, J., Guyon-Harris, K., Harrison, L., & Huth-Bocks, A. (2019). Effects of intimate partner violence and home environment on child language development in the first 3 years of life. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 40(2), 112121. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/dbp.000000000000063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence two-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 18341849. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01660.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putman, M., & Walker, C. (2010). Motivating children to read and write: Using informal learning environments as contexts for literacy instruction. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 24(2), 140151. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568541003635243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879891. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., and Raven, J. (1995). Raven’s coloured progressive matrices. Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Renninger, K. A. (2000). Individual interest and its implications for understanding intrinsic motivation. In Sansone, C. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 373404). Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riches, C., & Curdt-Christiansen, X. L. (2010). A tale of two Montréal communities: Parents’ perspectives on their children’s language and literacy development in a multilingual context. Canadian Modern Language Review, 66(4), 525555. https://doi.org/10.3138/cmlr.66.4.525CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodriguez, E. T., & Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S. (2011). Trajectories of the home learning environment across the first 5 years: Associations with children’s vocabulary and literacy skills at prekindergarten. Child Development, 82(4), 10581075. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01614.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ronderos, J., Castilla-Earls, A., & Marissa Ramos, G. (2021). Parental beliefs, language practices and language outcomes in Spanish–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 122. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2021.1935439Google ScholarPubMed
San San Kyaw, K., Tin Tin, S., Underwood, L., & Grant, C. (2019). Effects of home language environment and household crowding on early expressive language development. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 1.https://doi.org/10.1097/dbp.0000000000000764Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic childrenChild Development61(6), 17281743. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130834CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14(3), 245302. https://doi.org/10.1006/DREV.1994.1010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S., Neuman, S., & Dickinson, D. (2009). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis) abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. Fletcher-Campbell, F., Soler, J., & Reid, G. (Eds), Approaching difficulties in literacy development: Assessment, pedagogy and programmes (p.2338). Sage.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning, and motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26(3–4), 299323. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.1991.9653136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A 5-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445460. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Continuity and change in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading. Child Development, 85(4), 15521568. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder-McLean, L. K., & McLean, J. E. (1978). Verbal information gathering strategies: The child’s use of language to acquire languageJournal of Speech and Hearing Disorders43(3), 306325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Son, S. H., & Morrison, F. J. (2010). The nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Developmental psychology, 46(5), 1103. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 934947. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, H., Yin, B., Amsah, N. F. B. B., & O’brien, B. A. (2018). Differential effects of internal and external factors in early bilingual vocabulary learning: The case of Singapore. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(2), 383411. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700039XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tse, S. K., Zhu, Y., Hui, Y. S., & Ng, H. W. (2017). The effects of home reading activities during preschool and Grade 4 on children’s reading performance in Chinese and English in Hong Kong. Australian Journal of Education, 61(1), 523. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004944116689093Google Scholar
van der Schuit, M., Segers, E., van Balkom, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2011). How cognitive factors affect language development in children with intellectual disabilitiesResearch in Developmental Disabilities32(5), 18841894. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.03.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing. 16(1), 520. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021789804977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weigel, D. J., Martin, S. S., & Bennett, K. K. (2006). Mothers’ literacy beliefs: Connections with the home literacy environment and pre-school children’s literacy development. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 6(2), 191211. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798406066444CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., &Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69(3), 848872. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06247.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test (2nd ed.). Pearson AssessmentsGoogle Scholar
Yeung, S. S., & King, R. B. (2016). Home literacy environment and English language and literacy skills among Chinese young children who learn English as a second language. Reading Psychology, 37(1), 92120. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2015.1009591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, X., & Lau, C. (2022). Development and validation of a parent belief and attitude questionnaire on supporting young children’s English as a second/foreign language development. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 119. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2022.2105854Google Scholar
Zhang, X., Lau, C., & Su, Y. (2021). Home environment and development of English as a second/foreign language for young children in Asian contexts: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Early Education and Development, 132. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2021.1981065Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Figure 1

Figure 1. The Model Depicting Relations Between Home Environment, Interest, and Young Chinese Children’s English Language Development.Notes. *p < .05; **p < .01; Lan = English language outcomes; RV = receptive vocabulary; EV = expressive vocabulary; PIH = Parents’ perceived importance of the home environment; CES = Parents’ perceived child’s English ability and self-efficacy; CB = control beliefs; HEA = Home English activities; HL = home language use; BR = English book resources; SES = family SES; CI = children’s interest in learning English; Children’s gender, non-verbal intelligence, and the type of kindergartens were controlled in the model.

Figure 2

Table 2. Correlations for the home environment, interest, and English language development

Figure 3

Table 3. Total, direct, and indirect effects of parental beliefs and family SES on English language development