Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T04:25:02.430Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infants' gestures influence mothers' provision of object, action and internal state labels*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 February 2011

JANET OLSON*
Affiliation:
Northern Illinois University
ELISE FRANK MASUR
Affiliation:
Northern Illinois University
*
Address for correspondence: Janet Olson, School of Allied Health & Communicative Disorders, Northern Illinois, University, DeKalb, IL 60115. tel: 815-753-1401; fax : 815-753-9123; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Twenty-four infants at 1 ; 1 and their mothers were videotaped for 18 minutes while playing. Infants' pointing, reaching and object-extending gestures were coded in three communicative intent contexts: proto-declarative, or commenting; proto-imperative, or requesting; and ambiguous. Mothers' responses to infants' gestures were coded as object labels, action labels, internal state labels and non-labeling utterances. Infants most often pointed in the proto-declarative and used object extensions in the proto-imperative context. Infants produced pointing and reaching equivalently in the ambiguous context. Mothers' responses included object labels more often in response to points than object extensions. In contrast, mothers provided action labels most often in response to object extensions. Mothers produced large proportions of internal state labels, although the type varied by gesture. Results suggest mothers' labels following infants' gestures may serve as a mechanism for vocabulary acquisition and internal state understanding.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Analyses in this study are based on the dissertation research of the first author. Portions of the results were presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Baltimore, March, 2010. The authors thank Emily Block and Grace Wybourn for assistance coding; Jihee Hong, Carrie Lloyd, Leigh Mangun and Trish Schnell for assistance collecting and managing data; and the participating children and mothers.

References

REFERENCES

Acredolo, L. & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 59, 450–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A. & Markman, E. M. (1989). Establishing word-object relations: A first step. Child Development 60(2), 381–98.Google ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21(3), 205226.Google Scholar
Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Bornstein, M. H. (1997). Maternal verbal sensitivity and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development 20(2), 247–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, J., O'Rourke, P. & Borzellino, G. (1994). Form and function in the development of pointing and reaching gestures. Infant Behavior and Development 17, 195203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, J., Vitale, G., Osborne, P. & Olshansky, E. (2005). A cross-cultural comparison of communicative gestures in human infants during the transition to language. Gesture 5, 201217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Margulis, C., Tinker, E. & Fujita, N. (1996). Early conversations and word learning: Contributions from child and adult. Child Development 67, 3154–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O. & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language 25, 367–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornsetin, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. and Haynes, M. (1999). First words in the second year: Continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Infant Behavior and Development 22(1), 6585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language 35, 207220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camaioni, L., Castelli, M. C., Longobardi, E. & Volterra, V. (1991). A parent report instrument for early language assessment. First Language 11, 345–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (Serial 255), 63(4) 1174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, R., Mastergeorge, A. & Coggins, T. (1983). The acquisition of communicative intentions in infants 8 to 15 months of age. Language and Speech 26(2), 101116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crais, E., Douglas, D. D. & Campbell, C. C. (2004). The intersection of the development of gestures and intentionality. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47, 678–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C. & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development 62(6), 1352–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Reznick, J. & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur Communicative Developmental Inventories: User's guide and technical manual, 2nd edn.Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Franco, F. & Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language 23, 307336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldfield, B. A. & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language 17, 171–83.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2002). Constructing communication by hand. Cognitive Development 17, 1385–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Pointing sets the stage for learning language and creating language. Child Development 78(3), 741–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E. & Iverson, J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science 10(6), 778–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Morford, M. (1985). Gesture in early child language. Merrill Palmer Quarterly 31(2), 145–76.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Wagner, S. (2005). How our hands help us learn. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(5), 234–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harding, C. & Golinkoff, R. (1979). The origins of intentional vocalizations in prelinguistic infants. Child Development 50(1), 3340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howell, D. C. (1987). Statistical methods for psychology, 2nd edn.Boston: Duxbury.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16(5), 367–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T. & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science 7(3), 297307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcos, H. (1991). How adults contribute to the development of early referential communication? European Journal of Psychology of Education 6(3), 271–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masur, E. (1982). Mothers' responses to infants' object-related gestures: Influences on lexical development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 9, 2330.Google ScholarPubMed
Masur, E. (1983). Gestural development, dual-directional signaling, and the transition to words. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 12(2), 93–109.Google Scholar
Masur, E. (1987). Imitative interchanges in a social context: Mother–infant matching behavior at the beginning of the second year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33(4) 453–72.Google Scholar
Masur, E. & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants' spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: Relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 48, 405426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masur, E. F., Flynn, V. & Eichorst, D. (2005). Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development. Journal of Child Language 32, 6391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E. & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development 73(6), 1715–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38(1–2), Serial #149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D. & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development 76(4), 763–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rollins, P. (2003). Caregivers' contingent comments to 9-month-old infants: Relationships with later language. Applied Psycholinguistics 24, 221–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M. L. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009a). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science 12(1), 182–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009b). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 323, 951–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbagh, M. & Callanan, M. (1998). Metarepresentation in action: Three-, 4-, and 5-year olds' developing theories of mind in parent–child conversations. Developmental Psychology 34(3), 491502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C. & Carpenter, M. (2008). Maternal talk about mental states and the emergence of joint visual attention. Infancy 13(6), 640–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C. & Carpenter, M. (2009). Maternal mental state talk and infants' early gestural communication. Journal of Child Language 36, 1053–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taumoepeau, M. & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development 77, 465–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taumoepeau, M. & Ruffman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others' minds: Maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24, and 33 months. Child Development 79, 284302.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Pragmatic contexts for verb learning. In Tomasello, M. & Merriman, W. E., Beyond names for things, 115–46. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M. & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Developmental Psychology 78(3), 705722.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57, 1454–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wetherby, A., Cain, D., Yonclas, D. & Walker, V. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 31(2), 240–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, J., Hadley, P. & Ashland, (1996). Communication and language development in infants and toddlers. In. Hanson, M. (ed.), Atypical infant development, 365402. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J., McCathren, R. B., Warren, S. F. & Watson, A. L. (2001). Important distinctions in measuring maternal responses to communication in prelinguistic children with disabilities. Communication Disorders Quarterly 22(3), 135–47.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J. & Warren, S. (1998). Maternal responsivity predicts the prelinguistic communication intervention that facilitates generalized intentional communication. Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing Research 41, 1207–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed