Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T06:39:05.159Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The impact of positive parenting behaviors and maternal depression on the features of young children's home language environments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2019

Amy E. TREAT*
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, USA
Amanda SHEFFIELD MORRIS
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, USA
Jennifer HAYS-GRUDO
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, USA
Amy C. WILLIAMSON
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Oklahoma State University – HDFS, 700 North Greenwood Main Hall, Tulsa Oklahoma74070, United States. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study investigated the associations between maternal depression when infants were 3 to 11 months old (M = 6 months), and positive parenting behaviors when children were between 12 and 22 months (M = 17 months) and the home language environment assessed when children were 18 to 28 months old (M = 23.5 months) in a sample of 29 low-income mother–child dyads. After controlling for maternal education, only teaching behaviors remained a moderate and significant predictor of adult word counts. Observed teaching behaviors significantly predicted conversational turns and marginally predicted child vocalizations; effects sizes were small. Encouraging behaviors were a small and significant predictor of conversational turns and a marginally significant predictor of adult word counts. Maternal depression was a moderate and significant predictor of children's vocal productivity scores and a small, marginal predictor of conversational turns. These findings have important implications for parenting and children's language outcomes.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beebe, B., Jaffe, J., Buck, K., Chen, H., Cohen, P., Feldstein, S., & Andrews, H. (2008). Six-week postpartum maternal depressive symptoms and 4-month mother–infant self-and interactive contingency. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of The World Association for Infant Mental Health, 29(5), 442–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becerra, R. M. (1998). The Mexican-American family. In Mindel, C. H., Havenstein, R. W., & Wright, R. (Eds.), Ethnic families in America: patterns and variations (pp. 153–71). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Bell, B., Bradley, R. H., Stallard, N., & Stewart-Brown, S. L. (2007). Socioeconomic risk, parenting during the preschool years and child health age 6 years. European Journal of Public Health, 17(5), 508–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L. (2012). Cognitive and socioemotional caregiving in developing countries. Child Development, 83(1), 4661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Morgan, J., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., Arseneault, L. et al. (2004). Maternal expressed emotion predicts children's antisocial behavior problems: using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 149–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three-month-old infants’ reaction to simulated maternal depression. Child Development, 54(1), 185–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1024–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du, S., Xu, D., Richards, J. A., Hannon, S. M., & Gilkerson, J. (2017). The LENA™ Vocal Productivity Measure. Boulder, CO: LENA Foundation. Retrieved from <http://www.lena.org/wp-content/uploads/pdf/technical-reports/LTR-11-1_Vocal%20Productivity.pdf>.Google Scholar
Eaton, W. W., Smith, C., Ybarra, M., Muntaner, C., & Tien, A. (2004). Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale: Review and Revision (CESD and CESD-R). In Maruish, M. E. (Ed.), The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment: instruments for adults (pp. 363–77). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Feldman, R. (2009). The development of regulatory functions from birth to 5 years: insights from premature infants. Child Development, 80(2), 544–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiester, L. (2010). Early warning! Why reading by the end of third grade matters. KIDS COUNT special report. Annie E. Casey Foundation. Online <https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED509795>.Google Scholar
Fuligni, A. S., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2013). Mother–child interactions in Early Head Start: age and ethnic differences in low-income dyads. Parenting, 13(1), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gann, C., Bowers, L., & Walton, T. (2018). Small area income and poverty estimates: 2017 (SAIPE) program. December, 2018 - (P30-04). Retrieved from <https://www.census.gov/library/publications/2018/demo/p30-04.html>>Google Scholar
Gilkerson, J. (2017). Pathways to Opportunity. LENA Research Conference Beaver Creek, CO, September, 2017.Google Scholar
Gilkerson, J., & Richards, J. A. (2008). The LENA natural language study (Technical Report LTR-02-2). Boulder, CO: LENA Foundation. Retrieved from <https://www.lena.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/07/LTR-02-2_Natural_Language_Study.pdf>.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1983). Infant social cognition: self, people, and objects. In Liben, L. (Ed.), Piaget and the foundations of knowledge (pp. 179200). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Hoff, E., Rowe, M. L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2019). Language matters: denying the existence of the 30-million-word gap has serious consequences. Child Development, 90(3), 985–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., … & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children's language success. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1071–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Society for Research in Child Development Monograph Series. Serial no. 262, 65(3).Google ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaminski, J. W., Perou, R., Visser, S. N., Scott, K. G., Beckwith, L., Howard, J., … & Danielson, M. L. (2013). Behavioral and socioemotional outcomes through age 5 years of the legacy for children public health approach to improving developmental outcomes among children born into poverty. American Journal of Public Health, 103(6), 1058–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, P. S., Danko, C. M., Everhart, K. D., Diaz, A., Asherin, R. M., Vogeli, J. M., & Fekri, S. M. (2014). Maternal depression and expressive communication in one-year-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 37(3), 398405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(15), 9096–101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 627–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leffel, K., & Suskind, D. (2013). Parent-directed approaches to enrich the early language environments of children living in poverty. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 267–78.Google ScholarPubMed
Leigh, P., Nievar, M. A., & Nathans, L. (2011). Maternal sensitivity and language in early childhood: a test of the transactional model. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 113(1), 281–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O'Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behavior: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 20(5), 561–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsunaga, M. (2010). How to factor-analyze your data right: do's, don'ts, and how-to's. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(1), 97110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minkovitz, C. S., Hughart, N., Strobino, D., Scharfstein, D., Grason, H., Hou, W., … & Guyer, B. (2003). A practice-based intervention to enhance quality of care in the first 3 years of life: the Healthy Steps for Young Children Program. Jama, 290(23), 3081–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mittal, P. (2011). Centering parenting: pilot implementation of a group model for teaching family medicine residents well-child care. Permanente Journal, 15(4), 40–1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oller, D. K., Buder, E. H., Ramsdell, H. L., Warlaumont, A. S., Chorna, L., & Bakeman, R. (2013). Functional flexibility of infant vocalization and the emergence of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(16), 6318–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 285308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perou, R., Elliott, M. N., Visser, S. N., Claussen, A. H., Scott, K. G., Beckwith, L. H., … & Smith, D. C. (2012). Legacy for Children™: a pair of randomized controlled trials of a public health model to improve developmental outcomes among children in poverty. BMC Public Health, 12, 691. Online <https://bmcpublichealth.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1471-2458-12-691>.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pungello, E. P., Iruka, I. U., Dotterer, A. M., Mills-Koonce, R., & Reznick, J. S. (2009). The effects of socioeconomic status, race, and parenting on language development in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 544–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roggman, L. A., Cook, G. A., Innocenti, M. S., Jump Norman, V., & Christiansen, K. (2013). Parenting interactions with children: checklist of observations linked to outcomes (PICCOLO) in diverse ethnic groups. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34(4), 290306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roggman, L. A., Langlois, J. H., & Hubbs-Tait, L. (1987). Mothers, infants, and toys: social play correlates of attachment. Infant Behavior and Development, 10(2), 233–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romeo, R. R., Leonard, J. A., Robinson, S. T., West, M. R., Mackey, A. P., Rowe, M. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2018). Beyond the 30-million-word gap: children's conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychological Science, 29(5), 700–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85(3), 956–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. (2018). Understanding socioeconomic differences in parents’ speech to children. Child Development Perspectives, 12(2), 122–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmit, S., Golden, O., & Beardslee, W. (2014). Maternal depression: why it matters to an anti-poverty agenda for parents and children. Washington, DC: Center for Law and Social Policy.Google Scholar
Sohr-Preston, S. L., & Scaramella, L. V. (2006). Implications of timing of maternal depressive symptoms for early cognitive and language development. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 9(1), 6583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, A., Malmberg, L. E., Sylva, K., Barnes, J., Leach, P., & the FCCC team (2008). The influence of maternal depression, caregiving, and socioeconomic status in the post-natal year on children's language development. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(5), 603–12.Google ScholarPubMed
Suskind, D. L., Leffel, K. R., Graf, E., Hernandez, M. W., Gunderson, E. A., Sapolich, S. G., … & Levine, S. C. (2016). A parent-directed language intervention for children of low socioeconomic status: a randomized controlled pilot study. Journal of Child Language, 43(2), 366406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talking is Teaching (2017). Talk, read, sing, community campaign guide: too small to fail. Retrieved from <https://talkingisteaching.org/communities/resources/community-campaign-guide-2017-edition>..>Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Maternal responsiveness and early language acquisition. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 29, 89127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dam, N. T., & Earleywine, M. (2011). Validation of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale–Revised (CESD-R): pragmatic depression assessment in the general population. Psychiatry Research, 186(1), 128–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vericker, T., Macomber, J., & Golden, O. A. (2010). Infants of depressed mothers living in poverty: opportunities to identify and serve. Washington, DC: Urban Institute.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. In Gauvain, M. & Cole, M. (Eds.), Readings on the development of children (pp. 3441). New York: Worth.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Lexical output as related to children's vocabulary acquisition: effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 265–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, D., Yapanel, U., & Gray, S. (2009). Reliability of the LENA Language Environment Analysis System in young children's natural home environment. Boulder, CO: LENA Foundation. Retrieved from <https://www.lena.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/07/LTR-05-2_Reliability.pdf>.Google Scholar