Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:08:25.818Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How shall a deceptive thing be called?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

William E. Merriman*
Affiliation:
Kent State University
Lorna H. Jarvis
Affiliation:
Kent State University
John M. Marazita
Affiliation:
Kent State University
*
Department of Psychology, Kent State University, Kent, OH 44242, USA.

Abstract

Thirty-six three-, four- and five-year-olds were asked to select labels for deceptive stimuli (e.g. for an eraser that looked like a pencil). Three types of labelling were investigated –simple (e.g. ‘is an eraser’); appearance-predicated (e.g. ‘looks like an eraser’); and reality-predicated (e.g. ‘is really and truly an eraser’). An age-related appearance-reality shift was observed in simple labelling (e.g. older children were more likely than younger ones to accept eraser and reject pencil as simple names for the pencil-eraser). This trend was robust over method and semantic domain, though weaker with object than with colour labels. As in previous research, older children were more likely than younger ones to map different appearance- than reality-predicated labels onto an item (e.g. to accept that the pencil-eraser looks like a pencil, but is really and truly an eraser); however, all age groups were reluctant to extend more than one name to a stimulus via a common predicate (e.g. to accept two reality-predicated labels for the same object). This one-label-per-predicate pattern was observed more frequently within reality than within appearance predicates; more frequently with colour than with object names, and with questions blocked by predicate than by name. It is argued that younger children maintained this pattern because of inflexible encoding, but that older ones did so because of better understanding of the appearance-reality distinction, greater reality dominance, and a Mutual Exclusivity bias.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by an NIH FIRST Award (R29 HD 25958) to the first author. We are grateful to the children and parents of the Akron area for their participation. The assistance of Laura Augustajtis, Denise Bellis, Andrea Culver, Chris Derry, Jennifer Engar, Lisa Heffelfinger, Rebecca Katzenmeyer, Angela Lavorata, Brenda Riccio, Paul Scott, Barbara Sherwood, and Charlene Walkulski with various aspects of the research was much appreciated. ‘In all your ways acknowledge Him…’ (Prov. 3:6).

References

REFERENCES

Au, T. K. & Glusman, M. (1990). The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: to honor or not to honor? Child Development 61, 1474–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. W. (1958). How shall a thing be called? Pyschological Review 65, 1421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973). What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R. & Green, F. L. (1983). Development of the appearance-reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology 15, 95120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L. & Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 51 (1, Serial No. 212).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L. & Flavell, E. R. (1989). Young children's ability to differentiate appearance-reality and level 2 perspectives in the tactile modality. Child Development 60, 201–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, V. C. (1987). The contrastive hypothesis for the acquisition of word meaning: a reconsideration of the theory. Journal of Child Language 14, 493532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D. (1978). What looks like a jiggy but acts like a zimbo? A study of early word meaning using artificial objects. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 15, 16.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bailey, L. M. & Wenger, N. R. (1992). Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn new nouns. Developmental Psychology 28, 99108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchinson, J. E. (1986). Children's sensitivity to the contrastive use of object category terms. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 25, 4955.Google Scholar
Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Landau, B., Smith, L. B. & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development 3, 299321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Markman, E. M. & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology 20, 121–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, W. E. (1986). Some reasons for the occurrence and eventual correction of children's naming errors. Child Development 57, 942–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merriman, W. E. (1991). The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning: a reply to Woodward & Markman. Developmental Review 11, 164–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merriman, W. E. & Bowman, L. L. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 54 (3–4, Serial No. 220).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, W. E. & Schuster, J. M. (1991). Young children's disambiguation of object name reference. Child Development 62, 1288–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, W. E., Scott, P. & Marazita, J. (1993). An appearance-function shift in young children's object naming. Journal of Child Language 20, 101–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Catlin, J. & Rosch, E. (1975). Development of the structure of color categories. Developmental Psychology 11, 5460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B. & Pani, J. R. (1980). Acquisition of basic object categories. Cognitive Psychology 12, 496522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1988). Constraints on word learning? Cognitive Development 3, 221–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In Rosch, E. H. & Lloyd, B. (eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M. & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8, 382439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vincent-Smith, L., Bricker, D. & Bricker, W. (1974). Acquisition of receptive vocabulary in the toddler-age child. Child Development 45, 189–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. L. & Markman, E. M. (1991). Constraints on learning as default assumptions: comments on Merriman & Bowman's ‘The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning’. Developmental Review 14, 5777.Google Scholar