Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:30:10.308Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How children learn to produce appropriate referring expressions in narratives: the role of clarification requests and modeling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2017

Ana M. CARMIOL*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Costa Rica
Danielle MATTHEWS
Affiliation:
University of Sheffield
Odir A. RODRÍGUEZ-VILLAGRA
Affiliation:
Universidad de Costa Rica
*
Address for correspondence: Ana M. Carmiol, Instituto de Investigaciones Psicológicas, Universidad de Costa Rica, Código Postal 2060, San José, Costa Rica. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Asking children to clarify themselves promotes their ability to uniquely identify objects in referential communication tasks. However, little is known about whether parents ask preschoolers for clarification during interactions and, if so, how. Study 1 explored how mothers clarify their preschoolers’ ambiguous descriptions of the characters in their narratives, and whether clarification requests affect children's repairs of their ambiguous descriptions. Mothers were found to use different strategies, including signaling misunderstanding and modeling appropriate descriptions. Presence of these different strategies predicted children's ability to provide informative repairs. Study 2 tested the effect of children's experience with signaling misunderstanding and modeling on their ability to uniquely identify the characters of a story on a second narration. Experiencing modeling, but not misunderstandings, positively affected children's provision of appropriate descriptions during second narrations. Findings are discussed in terms of the role of imitation in driving referential development.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

We would to thank the participating mothers and children. We thank Siu-lin Rawlinson and Lina Jiménez for drawing and animating the story books. This research was supported by grants from Vicerrectoría de Investigación, Universidad de Costa Rica (723-A9-305, 723-B2-303) to the first author and from The British Academy of Science (LA100133) to the second and first author.

References

Ateş-Şen, B., & Küntay, A. (2015). Children's sensitivity to caregiver cues and the role of adult feedback in the developmentof referential communication. In Serratrice, L., & Allen, S. E. M. (Eds.), The acquisition of reference (pp. 241–62). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bannard, C., Klinger, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). How selective are 3-year-olds in imitating novel linguistic material? Developmental Psychology, 49, 2344–56.Google Scholar
Carmiol, A. M., & Vinden, P. G. (2013). Enhancing preschoolers’ understanding of ambiguity in communication: a training study on misunderstandings. Merrill Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 59, 79105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. J. (2003). Communicating complex information: the interpretation of statistical interaction in multiple logistic regression analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 93(9), 1376–a.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. G., & Snow, C. (1996). Five-year-olds’ interactions with fathers versus mothers. First Language, 16, 223–42.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M., & Hendriks, H. (1999). Cohesion and anaphora in children's narratives: a comparison of English, French, German, and Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 26, 419–52.Google Scholar
Horst, J. S., Parsons, K. L., & Bryan, N. M. (2011). Get the story straight: contextual repetition promotes word learning from storybooks. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00017Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). Some fundamental aspects of language development after age 5. In Fletcher, J., & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition: studies in first language development (pp. 455–74). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Klinger, J., Mayor, J., & Bannard, C. (2016), Children's faithfulness in imitating language use varies cross-culturally, contingent on prior experience. Child Development, 87, 820–33.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7, 297307.Google Scholar
Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: a training study. Child Development, 74, 1130–44.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: effects of distracters and feedback on referential communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 184210.Google Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: a training study. Child Development, 78, 1744–59.Google Scholar
McDermott Sales, J. M., Fivush, R., & Peterson, C. (2003). Parental reminiscing about positive and negative events. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4(2), 185209.Google Scholar
Menig-Peterson, C. L. (1975). The modification of communicative behavior in preschool-aged children as a function of the listener's perspective. Child Development, 46, 1015–18.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., & Sperry, L. (1988). Early talk about the past: the origins of conversational stories of personal experience. Journal of Child Language, 15, 293315.Google Scholar
Nilsen, E., & Mangal, L. (2012). Which is important for preschoolers’ production and repair of statements: what the listener knows or what the listener says? Journal of Child Language, 39, 1121–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Narrative skills of children with communication disorders. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 287313.Google Scholar
Norbury, C. F., Gemmel, T., & Paul, R. (2014). Pragmatic abilities in narrative production: a cross-disorder comparison. Journal of Child Language, 41, 485510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, R. J. D., & Dal Martello, M. F. (1986). The use of the definite and indefinite articles by Italian preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 13, 145–54.Google Scholar
Robinson, E. J., & Robinson, W. P. (1985). Teaching children about verbal referential communication. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8, 285–99.Google Scholar
Sénéchal, M. (1997). The differential effect of storybook reading on preschoolers’ acquisition of expressive and receptive vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 24(1), 123–38.Google Scholar
Smeets, D. J. H., & Bus, G. (2014). The interactive animated e-book as a word learning device for kindergartners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 122.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1981). The uses of imitation. Journal of Child Language, 8, 205–12.Google Scholar
Wagner, L., Kako, E., Amick, E., Carrigan, E., & Liu, K. (2005). Children's use of pointing to anchor reference during story-telling. In Brugos, A., Clark-Cotton, M. R., & Ha, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 639–50). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Want, S. C., & Harris, P. L. (2002). How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of ‘imitation’ in children. Developmental Science, 5, 114.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions B, 364, 2417–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkinson, K. S., & Houston-Price, C. (2013). Once upon a time, there was a pulchritudinous princess …: the role of word definitions and multiple story contexts in children's learning of difficult vocabulary. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(3), 591613.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. W., Muñoz-Sandoval, A. F., Ruef, M., & Alvarado, C. G. (2005). Woodcock Muñoz Language Survey—Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar