Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T21:14:31.007Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gesture and speech during shared book reading with preschoolers with specific language impairment*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2015

MANUELA LAVELLI*
Affiliation:
University of Verona
CHIARA BARACHETTI
Affiliation:
University of Verona
ELENA FLORIT
Affiliation:
University of Verona
*
Address for correspondence: Manuela Lavelli, Department of Philosophy, Education, and Psychology, University of Verona, via S. Francesco 22, 37129 Verona, Italy; e-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

This study examined (a) the relationship between gesture and speech produced by children with specific language impairment (SLI) and typically developing (TD) children, and their mothers, during shared book-reading, and (b) the potential effectiveness of gestures accompanying maternal speech on the conversational responsiveness of children. Fifteen preschoolers with expressive SLI were compared with fifteen age-matched and fifteen language-matched TD children. Child and maternal utterances were coded for modality, gesture type, gesture–speech informational relationship, and communicative function. Relative to TD peers, children with SLI used more bimodal utterances and gestures adding unique information to co-occurring speech. Some differences were mirrored in maternal communication. Sequential analysis revealed that only in the SLI group maternal reading accompanied by gestures was significantly followed by child's initiatives, and when maternal non-informative repairs were accompanied by gestures, they were more likely to elicit adequate answers from children. These findings support the ‘gesture advantage’ hypothesis in children with SLI, and have implications for educational and clinical practice.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Manuela Lavelli, Chiara Barachetti, Elena Florit, Department of Philosophy, Education, and Psychology, University of Verona, Italy; Elena Florit is now at the Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, Italy. This work is part of a National Research Project Prin-2008, co-funded for a biennial period (2010–2012) by the Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research, and the University of Verona, to Manuela Lavelli. The authors are grateful to all the mothers, the children, and the speech–language therapists who participated in the study.

References

REFERENCES

Acredolo, L. & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 59, 450–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakeman, R. & Quera, V. (1995). Analyzing interaction: sequential analysis with SDIS & GSEQ. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barachetti, C. & Lavelli, M. (2011). Responsiveness of children with specific language impairment and maternal repairs during shared book reading. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 46, 579–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T. & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioral marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37, 391403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, J., Myszczyszyn, D., Jokel, A. & Bebiroglu, N. (2008). Gestures accompanying speech in specifically language-impaired children and their timing with speech. First Language 28, 237–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botting, N., Riches, N., Gaynor, M. & Morgan, G. (2010). Gesture production and comprehension in children with specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 28, 5169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capone, N. C. (2007). Tapping toddlers’ evolving semantic representation via gesture. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50, 732–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselli, M. C., Pasqualetti, P. & Stefanini, S. (2007). Parole e frasi nel ‘Primo Vocabolario del Bambino’. Nuovi dati normative fra 18 e 36 mesi e Forma breve del questionario. Milano: Franco Angeli.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. S. (2000). Children's language learning: an interactionist perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 41, 3354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cianchetti, C. & Sannio Fancello, G. (1997). TVL/Test di Valutazione del Linguaggio. Trento: Erickson.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G. (1990). Maternal recast and other replies to language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 55, 262–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., Hutcheson, G. D. & Grove, J. (1995). Contingency and breakdown: children with SLI and their conversations with mothers and fathers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 1290–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cresti, E. & Moneglia, M. (1997). L'intonazione e i criteri di trascrizione del parlato. In Bortolini, U & Pizzuto, E. (eds), Il progetto CHILDES – Italia, vol. 2, 5790. Pisa: Del Cerro.Google Scholar
Crowe, L. K. (2000). Reading behaviors of mothers and their children with language impairment during repeated storybook reading. Journal of Communication Disorders 33, 503–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowe, L. K., Norris, J. A. & Hoffman, P. R. (2004). Training caregivers to facilitate communicative participation of preschool children with language impairment during storybook reading. Journal of Communication Disorders 37, 177–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis Weismer, S. E. & Hesketh, L. J. (1993). The influence of prosodic and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 10131025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W. & McNeil, N. M. (2001). Divergence of verbal expression and embodied knowledge: evidence from speech and gesture in children with specific language impairment. Language and Cognitive Processes 16, 309–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M. A. & Schmidt, F. (1991). Repeated maternal book reading with two children: language-normal and language impaired. First Language 11, 269–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S. & Reilly, J. S. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories (CDI). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics 21, 95115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language 29, 336–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girolametto, L., Bonifacio, S., Visini, C., Weitzman, E., Zocconi, E. & Pearce, P. S. (2002). Mother–child interactions in Canada and Italy: linguistic responsiveness to late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 37, 151–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Beyond words: the importance of gestures to researchers and learners. Child Development 71, 231–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E. & Iverson, J. M. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science 10, 778–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammett, L., van Kleeck, A. & Huberty, C. J. (2003). Patterns of parents’ extratextual interactions during book sharing with preschool children: a cluster analysis study. Reading Research Quarterly 38, 442–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammet Price, L., van Kleeck, A. & Huberty, C. J. (2009). Talk during book sharing between parents and preschool children: a comparison between storybook and expository book condition. Reading Research Quarterly 44, 171–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, E. L., Bishop, D. V. M. & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1998). Representational gestures in developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment: error-types and the reliability of ratings. Human Movement Sciences 17, 655–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Braddock, B. A. (2011). Gesture and motor skills in relation to language in children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 54, 7286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M. & Goldwin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16, 368–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M., Longobardi, E., Spampinato, K. & Caselli, M. C. (2006). Gesture and speech in maternal input to children with Down's syndrome. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 41, 235–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, J. R., Miller, J., Curtiss, S. & Tallal, P. (1993). Conversations with children who are language impaired: asking questions. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 973–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Justice, L. M. & Kaderavek, J. (2003). Topic control during shared storybook reading: mothers and their children with language impairments. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 23, 137–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J., Bowles, R. & Grimm, K. (2005). Language impairment, parent–child shared reading, and phonological awareness: a feasibility study. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 25, 143–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, E., Pine, K. J. & Ryder, N. (2011). I hear what you say but I see what you mean: the role of gestures in children's pragmatic comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes 26, 149–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Mainela-Arnold, E., Alibali, M. W., Ryan, K. & Evans, J. L. (2011). Knowledge of mathematical equivalence in children with specific language impairment: insights from gesture and speech. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 42, 1830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mainela-Arnold, E., Evans, J. L. & Alibali, M. W. (2006). Understanding conversation delays in children with specific language impairment: task representations revealed in speech and gesture. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 49, 1267–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, K. K., Rohlfing, K. J., Bean, A. & Marschner, E. (2009). Gesture as a support for word learning: the case of under. Journal of Child Language 36, 807–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: what gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, Vol. 1: Transcription Format and Programs, and Vol. 2: The Database. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (1983). Joint book reading as multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Developmental Psychology 19, 445–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özçalişkan, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition 96, B10113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pigem, N. & Blicharski, T. (2002). Les styles de participation des enfants de 5–6 ans au cours d'une lecture d'album. Enfance 54, 169–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor-Williams, K., Fey, M. & Loeb, D. F. (2001). Parental recasts and production of copulas and articles by children with specific language impairment and typical language. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology 10, 155–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabidoux, P. C. & MacDonald, J. D. (2000). An interactive taxonomy of mothers and children during storybook interactions. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology 9, 331–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E. & Cox, A. (1999). Quality of adult book reading affects children's emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology 35, 20–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riches, N., Faragher, B. & Conti-Ramsden, G. M. (2006). Verb schema use and input dependence in five-year-old children with SLI. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 41, 117–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skibbe, L. E., Moody, A. J., Justice, L. M. & McGinty, A. S. (2010). Socio-emotional climate of storybook reading interactions for mothers and preschoolers with language impairment. Reading & Writing 23, 5371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. & Goldfield, B. A. (1983). Turn the page please: situation-specific language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10, 551–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stefanini, S., Recchia, M. & Caselli, M. C. (2008). The relationship between spontaneous gesture production and spoken lexical ability in children with Down syndrome in a naming task. Gesture 8, 197218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sulzby, E. & Kaderavek, J. (1996). Parent–child language during storybook reading and toy play contexts: case studies of normally developing and specific language impaired (SLI) children. National Reading Conference Yearbook 45, 257–69.Google Scholar
Thal, D., Tobias, S. & Morrison, D. (1991). Language and gesture in late talkers: a one-year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34, 604–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2006). Acquiring linguistic constructions. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (eds) & Kuhn, D. & Siegler, R. S. (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language, 6th ed., 255–98. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Valenzeno, L., Alibali, M. W. & Klatzky, R. (2003). Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: a lesson in symmetry. Contemporary Educational Psychology 28, 187204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, X.-L., Bernas, R. & Eberhard, P. (2001). Effects of teachers’ verbal and non-verbal scaffolding on everyday classroom performances of students with Down syndrome. International Journal of Early Years Education 9, 7180.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1969). Scala Wechsler a livello prescolare e di scuola elementare. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.Google Scholar
Yaden, D. B. (2003). Parent–child storybook reading as a complex adaptive system: Or ‘An igloo is a house for bears’. In Van Kleeck, A., Stahl, S. A. & Bauer, B. (eds), On reading books to children, 336–62. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Yont, K. M., Hewitt, L. E. & Miccios, A. W. (2002). ‘What did you say?’ Understanding conversational breakdowns in children with speech and language impairments. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 16, 265–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed