Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T00:21:14.386Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

For and against frequencies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 February 2015

CHARLES YANG*
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bloch, B. (1947). English verb inflection. Language 23(4), 399418.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: form and function in emerging grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R., Cazden, C. B. & Bellugi, U. (1973). The child's grammar from I to III. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development (pp. 295333). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Aveledo, F., and Roca, I. (2002). The development of regular and irregular verb inflection in Spanish child language. Journal of Child Language 29(3), 591622.Google Scholar
Crain, S. & Thornton, R. (2000). Investigations in universal grammar: a guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grammer, K. & Thornhill, R. (1994). Human (homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: the role of symmetry and averageness. Journal of Comparative Psychology 108(3), 233242.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. (2004). Language acquisition: the growth of grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N. (2000). Domain specificity in face perception. Nature Neuroscience 3, 759763.Google Scholar
Kim, Y.-J. (2000). Subject/object drop in the acquisition of Korean: a cross-linguistic comparison. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 9(4), 325351.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1995). The phonological basis of sound change. In Goldsmith, J. (ed.), The handbook of phonological theory (pp.640670). Cambridge: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kowalski, A. & Yang, C. (2012). Verb islands in child and adult language. In Biller, A. K., Chung, E. Y. & Kimball, A. E. (eds), BUCLD 36: Proceedings of the 36th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, vol. 1 (pp. 281289). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1981). Resolving the neogrammarian controversy. Language 57(?), 267308.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1989). The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change 1(1), 8597.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1994). Principles of linguistic change: internal factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (2006). A sociolinguistic perspective on sociophonetic research. Journal of Phonetics 34(4), 500515.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (2014). The regularity of regular sound change. Unpublished manuscript, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Labov, W., Ash, S. & Boberg, C. (2006). The atlas of North American English: phonetics, phonology, and sound change. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W., Rosenfelder, I. & Fruehwald, J. (2013). One hundred years of sound change in Philadelphia: linear incrementation, reversal and re-analysis. Language 89(1), 3065.Google Scholar
Legate, J. A. & Yang, C. (2002). Empirical re-assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. Linguistic Review 19, 151162.Google Scholar
Legate, J. A. & Yang, C. (2007). Morphosyntactic learning and the development of tense. Language Acquisition 14(3), 315344.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. V., Pine, J. M. & Barnes, H. D. (1992). Individual differences in early vocabulary development: redefining the referential–expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language 19(2), 287310.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk, 3rd ed.Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Phillips, B. (1984). Word frequency and the actuation of sound change. Language 60(2), 320342.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. & Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition 28(1), 73193.Google Scholar
Roberts, J. & Labov, W. (1995). Learning to talk Philadelphian: acquisition of short a by preschool children. Language Variation and Change 7, 101112.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. & the PDP Research Group (eds), Parallel distributed processing: explorations into the microstructure of cognition. Volume 2: Psychological and biological models, 216271. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sakas, W. G. & Fodor, J. D. (2012). Disambiguating syntactic triggers. Language Acquisition 19(2), 83143.Google Scholar
Smith, J., Durham, M. & Fortune, L. (2009). Universal and dialect-specific pathways of acquisition: caregivers, children, and t/d deletion. Language Variation and Change 21(1), 6995.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J. (2011). A validation of amazon mechanical turk for the collection of acceptability judgments in linguistic theory. Behavior Research Methods 43(1), 155167.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J. & Almeida, D. (2012). Assessing the reliability of textbook data in syntax: Adger's Core Syntax. Journal of Linguistics 48(3), 609652.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cognitive Linguistics 11(1/2), 6182.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (1991). Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition 40(1/2), 2181.Google Scholar
Valian, V., Solt, S. & Stewart, J. (2009). Abstract categories or limited-scope formulae? The case of children's determiners. Journal of Child Language 36(4), 743778.Google Scholar
Wang, Q., Lillo-Martin, D., Best, C. T. & Levitt, A. (1992). Null subject versus null object: some evidence from the acquisition of Chinese and English. Language Acquisition 2(3), 221254.Google Scholar
Wexler, K. (1994). Optional infinitives, verb movement and the economy of derivation in child grammar. In Lightfoot, D. & Hornstein, N. (eds), Verb movement, 305350. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Xu, F. & Pinker, S. (1995). Weird past tense forms. Journal of Child Language 22(3), 531556.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (1999). A selectionist theory of language development. In Robert Dale & Kenneth W. Church (Eds.), Proceedings of ACL (pp. 429435). Stroudsburg, PA: Associaton for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2002). Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2004). Universal grammar, statistics or both? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(10), 451456.Google Scholar
Yang, C. (2013). Ontogeny and phylogeny of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110(16), 63246327.Google Scholar