Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:59:16.705Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The emergence of communicative competence: detection of conversational topics*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Mary A. Luszcz
Affiliation:
The Flinders University of South Australia
Verne R. Bacharach
Affiliation:
Acadia University

Abstract

The use of linguistic and extralinguistic information in identifying conversational topics was studied in 3- and 5-year-old children. Pictures portraying an actor–action–object relation were used to guide conversations. Both active pictures, emphasizing an action relating actor and object via postural cues, and static pictures, which did not, were used. Linguistic topics were implied by prefacing each picture with comments topicalizing actor, action or object. In neutral control groups no topic was implied and, in an explicit control group of 3-year-Olds the actor's action on the object was directly topicalized. While implicit topic definition was sufficient for 5-year-olds' production of contingent replies, explicit definition of a topic was necessary to evoke a similar pattern of replies from 3-year-olds. These findings are discussed in terms of different modes of topic definition and their order of acquisition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ackerman, B. P. (1978). Children's comprehension of presupposed information: logical and pragmatic inferences to speaker. JExpChPsych 26. 92114.Google Scholar
Bacharach, V. R. & Luszcz, M. A. (1979). Communicative competence in young children: the use of implicit linguistic information. ChDev 50. 260–3.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & MacWhinney, B. (1979). A functionalist approach to the acquisition of grammar. In Ochs, E. & Schiefflin, B. B. (eds), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult-child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. CogPsych 8. 521–52.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1970). Meaning and the structure of language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G. & de Villiers, P. (1978). Language acquisition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1977). Children's illocutionary acts. In Freedle, R. O. (ed.), Discourse production and comprehension. New Jersey: Ablex.Google Scholar
Friedman, S. C. & Stevenson, M. B. (1975). Developmental changes in the understanding of implied motion in two-dimensional pictures. ChDev 46. 773–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M. & Smith, J. H. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. & Charney, R. (1981). Conversational asymmetry between mothers and children. JChLang. 8. 183200.Google ScholarPubMed
Keenan, E. O. & Klein, E. (1975). Coherency in children's discourse. JPsycholingRes 4. 365–80.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (1978). Sentential devices for conveying givenness and newness: a cross-cultural developmental study. JVLVB 17. 539–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr.Soc.Res.Ch.Dev. 38.Google Scholar