Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:02:26.477Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of target attributes on children's patterns of referential under- and over-specification

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 July 2015

MONIQUE CHAREST*
Affiliation:
University of Alberta, Canada
JUDITH R. JOHNSTON
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia, Canada
*
Address for correspondence: Monique Charest, University of Alberta, 8205 114 Street, 2–70 Corbett Hall, Edmonton Alberta T6G 2G4, Canada. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We examined the effects of object attributes on children's descriptive patterns in a referential communication task. Thirty preschoolers described object pairs that were selected by the experimenter. The targets were defined by shared size or colour, and differed on the non-target dimension in half of the trials. The children also completed a non-verbal reasoning task with analogous stimuli. They selected objects after observing the experimenter make a choice and inferring the basis for selection. In the communication task, the children produced fewer size than colour descriptions, particularly when the size targets differed in colour. They also over-specified colour features more often than size. They did not show a similar challenge identifying size relationships in the non-verbal task. The results support the conclusion that target attributes have a systematic influence on children's referential performance. Potential mechanisms for these effects, and directions for future research are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Asher, S. R. (1979). Referential communication. In Whitehurst, G. J. & Zimmerman, B. (Eds.), The functions of language and cognition (pp. 175197). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belke, E. (2006). Visual determinants of preferred adjective order. Visual Cognition, 14, 261294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belke, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2002). Tracking the time course of multidimensional stimulus discrimination: analyses of viewing patterns and processing times during ‘same’ – ‘different’ decisions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 237266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., & Levelt, W. (1994). Language production: grammatical encoding. In Gernsbacher, M. A. (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 945984). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brown-Schmidt, S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2006). Watching the eyes when talking about size: an investigation of message formulation and utterance planning. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 592609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd ed.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S., & Fenson, L. (1996). Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 125127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T. (1982). Social interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition, 11, 159184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, V. S., & Slevc, L. R. (2007). Grammatical encoding. In Gaskell, M. G. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 453470). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ford, W., & Olson, D. (1975). The elaboration of the noun phrase in children's description of objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 371382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., & Ratterman, M. J. (1991). Language and the career of similarity. In Gelman, S. & Byrnes, J. (Eds.), Perspectives on thought and language: interrelations in development (pp. 225277). London: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J. R., & Smith, L. B. (1989). Dimensional thinking in language impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 3338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, J. R., Smith, L. B., & Box, P. (1997). Cognition and communication: referential strategies used by preschoolers with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 964974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: from intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D., McKee, C., & Garrett, M. F. (2010). Children's sentence planning: syntactic correlates of fluency variations. Journal of Child Language, 37, 5994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadig, A. S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13, 329336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsen, E. S., & Graham, S. A. (2009). The relations between children's communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 220249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pechmann, T. (1989). Incremental speech production and referential overspecification. Linguistics, 27, 89110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B. (1984). Young children's understanding of attributes and dimensions: a comparison of conceptual and linguistic measures. Child Development, 55, 363380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surian, L. (1995). Children's ambiguous utterances: a re-examination of processing limitations on production. Journal of Child Language, 22, 151169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viethen, H. A. E., Goudbeek, M. B., & Krahmer, E. J. (2012). The impact of colour difference and colour codability on reference production. In Miyake, N., Peebles, D., & Cooper, R. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Sapporo, Japan (pp. 10841098). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J. (1976). The development of communication: changes with age and modeling. Child Development, 47, 473482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., & Merkur, A. E. (1977). The development of communication: modeling and contrast failure. Child Development, 48, 9931001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., & Sonnenschein, S. (1978). The development of communication: attribute variation leads to contrast failure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 25, 490504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., & Sonnenschein, S. (1985). The development of communication: a functional analysis. In Whitehurst, G. J. (Ed.), Annals of child development, Vol. 2 (pp. 148). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., Sonnenschein, S., & Ianfolla, B. J. (1981). Learning to communicate from models: children confuse length with information. Child Development, 52, 507513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar