Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:04:43.121Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of semantic plausibility, syntactic complexity and n-gram frequency on children's sentence repetition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 July 2020

Kamila POLIŠENSKÁ*
Affiliation:
The University of Manchester, UK
Shula CHIAT
Affiliation:
City, University of London, UK
Jakub SZEWCZYK
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
Katherine E. TWOMEY
Affiliation:
The University of Manchester, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Kamila Polišenská Division of Human Communication, Development and Hearing, The University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PL, United Kingdom Phone: +44 0161 275 3369 Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Theories of language processing differ with respect to the role of abstract syntax and semantics vs surface-level lexical co-occurrence (n-gram) frequency. The contribution of each of these factors has been demonstrated in previous studies of children and adults, but none have investigated them jointly. This study evaluated the role of all three factors in a sentence repetition task performed by children aged 4–7 and 11–12 years. It was found that semantic plausibility benefitted performance in both age groups; syntactic complexity disadvantaged the younger group but benefitted the older group; while contrary to previous findings, n-gram frequency did not facilitate, and in a post-hoc analysis even hampered, performance. This new evidence suggests that n-gram frequency effects might be restricted to the highly constrained and frequent n-grams used in previous investigations, and that semantics and morphosyntax play a more powerful role than n-gram frequency, supporting the role of abstract linguistic knowledge in children's sentence processing.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbot-Smith, K., & Behrens, H. (2006). How known constructions influence the acquisition of other constructions: The German passive and future constructions. Cognitive Science, 30(6), 9951026. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambridge, B. (2019). Against stored abstractions: A radical exemplar model of language acquisition. First Language. doi:10.1177/0142723719869731Google Scholar
Ambridge, B. (2013). How do children restrict their linguistic generalizations? An (un-) grammaticality judgment study. Cognitive Science, 37(3), 508543. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambridge, B., Barak, L., Wonnacott, E., Bannard, C., & Sala, G. (2018). Effects of both preemption and entrenchment in the retreat from verb overgeneralization errors: Four reanalyses, an extended replication, and a meta-analytic synthesis. Collabra: Psychology, 4. http://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.133Google Scholar
Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 239273. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500091400049XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2012). Semantics versus statistics in the retreat from locative overgeneralization errors. Cognition, 123(2), 260279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnon, I., & Priva, U. C. (2013). More than words: The effect of multi-word frequency and constituency on phonetic duration. Language and Speech, 56(3), 349371. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830913484891CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnon, I., & Snider, N. (2010). More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(1), 6782. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children's repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science, 19(3), 241248. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02075.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. https://doi.org/doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1988). The ‘no negative evidence’ problem: How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar. Explaining Language Universals, 73101.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (2001). Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2006). From usage to grammar: The mind's response to repetition. Language, 82(4), 711733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234272. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiat, S. (2001). Mapping theories of developmental language impairment: Premises, predictions and evidence. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2–3), 113142. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Oxford, England: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e62. [target article: pp. 1–19]. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1500031XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culicover, P., & Jackendoff, R. S. (2005). Simpler Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & Chang, F. (2014). The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 369(1634), 20120394. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2005). A new look at the acquisition of relative clauses. Language, 81(4), 882906. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2005.0169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., O'Donnell, M. B., & Römer, U. (2014). The processing of verb-argument constructions is sensitive to form, function, frequency, contingency and prototypicality. Cognitive Linguistics 25(1), 5598. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2013-0031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., Simpson-Vlach, R. I. T. A., & Maynard, C. (2008). Formulaic language in native and second language speakers: Psycholinguistics, corpus linguistics, and TESOL. Tesol Quarterly, 42(3), 375396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A. (2004). The search for the object begins at the verb. Presented at The 29th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, November 4–7.Google Scholar
Frank, S. L., & Christiansen, M. H. (2018). Hierarchical and sequential processing of language: A response to: Ding, Melloni, Tian, and Poeppel (2017). Rule-based and word-level statistics-based processing of language: insights from neuroscience. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(9), 12131218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., Jones, G., & Gobet, F. (2015). Simulating the cross-linguistic pattern of Optional Infinitive errors in children's declaratives and Wh-questions. Cognition, 143, 6176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frizelle, P., & Fletcher, P. (2014). Relative clause constructions in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(2), 255264. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12070CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, A. E. (2003). Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 219224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2003). Sense and structure: Meaning as a determinant of verb subcategorization preferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(2), 281303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 15691579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, C. L., Dell, G. S., Benjamin, A. S., & Bannard, C. (2016). Part and whole linguistic experience affect recognition memory for multi-word sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 87, 3858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jefferies, E., Ralph, M. A. L., & Baddeley, A. D. (2004). Automatic and controlled processing in sentence recall: The role of long-term and working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 623643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jolsvai, H., McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2013). Meaning overrides frequency in idiomatic and compositional multi-word chunks. In Knauff, N., Pauen, M., Sebanz, N., & Wachsmuth, I. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 692697). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Kidd, E., & Bavin, E. L. (2002). English-speaking children's comprehension of relative clauses: Evidence for general-cognitive and language-specific constraints on development. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(6), 599617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidd, E., Brandt, S., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Object relatives made easy: A cross-linguistic comparison of the constraints influencing young children's processing of relative clauses. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(6), 860897. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960601155284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klem, M., Melby-Lervåg, M., Hagtvet, B., Lyster, S. A. H., Gustafsson, J. E., & Hulme, C. (2015). Sentence repetition is a measure of children's language skills rather than working memory limitations. Developmental Science, 18(1), 146154. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211240. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites (Vol. 1). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, Volume II: The Database (Vol. 2). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2017). Computational investigations of multi-word chunks in language learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 9(3), 637652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michel, J. B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., Google Books Team, Pickett, J. P., Hoiberg, D., Clancy, D., Norvig, P., Orwant, J., Pinker, S., Nowak, M. A., & Aiden, E. L. (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science, 331(6014), 176182. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1199644CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikolov, T., Sutskever, I., Chen, K., Corrado, G. S., & Dean, J. (2013). Distributed representations of words and phrases and their compositionality. In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (pp. 3111–3119). Proceedings of a meeting held December 5–8, 2013, Lake Tahoe, Nevada, United States.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., & Isard, S. (1963). Some perceptual consequences of linguistic rules. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 217228. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(63)80087-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, K., Hulme, C., Nag, S., & Snowling, M. J. (2015). Sentence repetition as a marker of language skills in children with dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(2), 203221. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monaghan, P., & Christiansen, M. H. (2010). Words in puddles of sound: Modelling psycholinguistic effects in speech segmentation. Journal of Child Language, 37(3), 545564. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990511CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onnis, L., & Thiessen, E. (2013). Language experience changes subsequent learning. Cognition, 126(2), 268284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polišenská, K., Chiat, S., Comer, A., & McKenzie, K. (2014). Semantic effects in sentence recall: The contribution of immediate vs delayed recall in language assessment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 52, 6577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polišenská, K., Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2015). Sentence repetition: What does the task measure? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 50(1), 106118. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riches, N. G. (2012). Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment: An investigation of underlying mechanisms. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 47(5), 499510. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00158.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riches, N. G. (2017). Complex sentence profiles in children with Specific Language Impairment: Are they really atypical? Journal of Child Language, 44, 269296. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000847CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, R., Morgan, J. L., & Allopenna, P. (1998). Phonological and acoustic bases for earliest grammatical category assignment: A cross-linguistic perspective. Journal of Child Language, 25(1), 169201. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000997003395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supasiraprapa, S. (2019). Frequency effects on first and second language compositional phrase comprehension and production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40(4), 9871017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szewczyk, J. M., Marecka, M., Chiat, S., & Wodniecka, Z. (2018). Nonword repetition depends on the frequency of sublexical representations at different grain sizes: Evidence from a multi-factorial analysis. Cognition, 179, 2336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1992). First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., Derwing, B., Libben, G., & Westbury, C. (2011). Processing advantages of lexical bundles: Evidence from self-paced reading and sentence recall tasks. Language Learning, 61(2), 569613. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00622.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valian, V., Prasada, S., & Scarpa, J. (2006). Direct object predictability: Effects on young children's imitation of sentences. Journal of Child Language, 33(2), 247269. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000906007392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Heuven, W. J., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 11761190. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.850521CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallan, A., Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2011). Evaluation of an Arabic sentence-repetition test for preschool children. Poster presented at the ASHA Convention 2011, San Diego.Google Scholar
Yuan, S., Fisher, C., Kandhadai, P., & Fernald, A. (2011). You can stipe the pig and nerk the fork: Learning to use verbs to predict nouns. In Danis, N., Mesh, K., & Sung, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 665677). Somerville, Massachusetts: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar