Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T21:31:36.808Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of age and language on co-speech gesture production: an investigation of French, American, and Italian children's narratives*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2014

JEAN-MARC COLLETTA
Affiliation:
Université de Grenoble, LIDILEM, St Martin d'Hères, France
MICHÈLE GUIDETTI
Affiliation:
Université Toulouse 2, Octogone ECCD, Toulouse, France
OLGA CAPIRCI
Affiliation:
CNR, GLADD, Rome, Italy
CARLA CRISTILLI
Affiliation:
University of Naples L'Orientale, Naples, Italy
OZLEM ECE DEMIR
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, Evanston, ILL, USA
RAMONA N. KUNENE-NICOLAS
Affiliation:
University of Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, RSA
SUSAN LEVINE
Affiliation:
University of Chicago, Department of Psychology, Chicago, ILL, USA

Abstract

The aim of this paper is to compare speech and co-speech gestures observed during a narrative retelling task in five- and ten-year-old children from three different linguistic groups, French, American, and Italian, in order to better understand the role of age and language in the development of multimodal monologue discourse abilities. We asked 98 five- and ten-year-old children to narrate a short, wordless cartoon. Results showed a common developmental trend as well as linguistic and gesture differences between the three language groups. In all three languages, older children were found to give more detailed narratives, to insert more comments, and to gesture more and use different gestures – specifically gestures that contribute to the narrative structure – than their younger counterparts. Taken together, these findings allow a tentative model of multimodal narrative development in which major changes in later language acquisition occur despite language and culture differences.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Data collection and analysis for this study was financed by the French National Research Agency (projet ANR-05-BLANC-0178-01 et -02). We thank Asela Reig Alamillo and Valerio De Angelis for helping us with data coding and analysis, and we thank children from Grenoble, Toulouse, Chicago, Rome, and Naples who took part in this study.

References

REFERENCES

Alibali, M., Evans, J., Hostetter, A., Ryan, K. & Mainela-Arnold, E. (2009). Gesture–speech integration in narrative: are children less redundant than adults? Gesture 9(3), 290311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnlund, D. C. (1975). Communicative style in two cultures. In Kendon, A., Harris, R. M. & Key, M. R. (eds), Organization in behaviour in face-to-face interaction, 427–56. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill Palmer Quarterly 21(3), 205–26.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (ed.) (2004). Language development across childhood and adolescence. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: a crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Blake, J. (2000). Roots to language – evolutionary and developmental precursors. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In Kita, S. (ed.), Pointing: where language, culture and cognition meet, 933. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Camaioni, L. (1993). The development of intentional communication. In Nadel, J. & Camaioni, L. (eds), New perspectives in early communicative development, 8296. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Capirci, O., Caselli, M. C. & De Angelis, V. (2010). Gesture for speaking and gesture for thinking in children with typical and atypical development: a new theoretical framework. In Riva, D. & Njiokiktjien, C. (eds), Brain lesion localization and developmental functions: basal ganglia, connecting systems, cerebellum, mirror neurons, 201–16 (Mariani Foundation Paediatric Neurology Series: 22). Montrouge: John Libbey Eurotext.Google Scholar
Capirci, O., Colletta, J.-M., Cristilli, C., De Angelis, V. & Graziano, M. (2010). Linguistic and gestural reference tracking in Italian and French children's narratives. Paper presented at the Fourth Conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies (ISGS), 25–30 July, European University Viadrina, Frankfurt/Oder.Google Scholar
Capirci, O., Cristilli, C., De Angelis, V. & Graziano, M. (2011). Learning to use gesture in narratives: developmental trends in formal and semantic gesture competence. In Stam, G. & Ishino, M. (eds), Integrating gestures: the interdisciplinary nature of gesture, 189200 (Gesture Studies, 4). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Capirci, O., Iverson, J., Pizzuto, E. & Volterra, V. (1996). Gesture and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language 23, 645–73.Google Scholar
Capirci, O. & Volterra, V. (2008). Gesture and speech: the emergence and development of a strong and changing partnership. Gesture 8(1), 2244.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 63(4), 255–83.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C. (1990). Communicative gestures and first words. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children, 5667. New York: Springer-Verlag (1994: 2nd ed., Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press).Google Scholar
Colletta, J.-M. (2004). Le développement de la parole chez l'enfant âgé de 6 à 11 ans: corps, langage et cognition. Hayen: Mardaga.Google Scholar
Colletta, J.-M. (2009). Comparative analysis of children's narratives at different ages: a multimodal approach. Gesture 9(1), 6197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colletta, J.-M., Kunene, R. N., Venouil, A., Kauffman, V. & Simon, J.-P. (2009). Multitrack annotation of child language and gestures. In Kipp, M., Martin, J.-C., Paggio, P. & Heylen, D. (eds), Multimodal corpora: from models of natural interactions to systems and applications, 5472 (LNAI 5509). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Colletta, J.-M. & Pellenq, C. (2009). Multimodal explanations in French children aged from 3 to 11 years. In Nippold, N. & Scott, C. (eds), Expository discourse in children, adolescents, and adults: development and disorders, 6397. New York: Psychology Press, Erlbaum, Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Colletta, J.-M., Pellenq, C. & Guidetti, M. (2010). Age-related changes in co-speech gesture and narrative: evidence from French children and adults. Speech Communication 52, 565–76.Google Scholar
Cristilli, C., Capirci, O. & Graziano, M. (2010). Le funzioni anaforiche della gestualità nel racconto dei bambini. In Pettorino, M., Giannini, A. & Dovetto, F. M. (eds), La Comunicazione parlata 3: Atti del terzo Congresso Internazionale del gruppo di studi sulla Comunicazione Parlata, Vol. I, 307–39. Naples: Università degli Studi di Napoli ‘l'Orientale’.Google Scholar
Demir, Ö. E., So, W., Özyürek, A. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Turkish- and English-speaking children display sensitivity to perceptual context in the referring expressions they produce in speech and gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes 27(6), 844–67.Google Scholar
De Ruiter, J. (2000). The production of gesture and speech. In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and gesture, 284311. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1989). L'expression des émotions. In Rimé, B. & Scherer, K. R. (eds), Les émotions, 183201. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing gesture: how our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Graziano, M. (2009). Le développement de la relation entre les compétences verbale et gestuelle dans la construction d'un texte narratif chez l'enfant âgé de 4 à 10 ans (unpublished PhD dissertation). Université Stendhal, Grenoble et Università degli Studî Suor Orsola Benincasa, Napoli.Google Scholar
Guidetti, M. (2002). The emergence of pragmatics: forms and functions of conventional gestures in young French children. First Language 22(3), 265–85.Google Scholar
Guidetti, M. (2005). Yes or no? How do young children combine gestures and words to agree and refuse. Journal of Child Language 32, 911–24.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M. (2006). Handling discourse: gestures, reference tracking, and communication strategies in early L2. Language Learning 56, 155–96.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M., Hendricks, H. & Hickmann, M. (2008). Learning to talk and gesture about motion in French. First Language 28(2), 200–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M. (2003). Children's discourse: person, space and time across languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M. & Hendricks, H. (1999). Cohesion and anaphora in children's narratives: a comparison of English, French, German, and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language 26, 419–52.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9, 2343.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Volterra, V. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: early communication in Italian vs. American children. First Language 28(2), 164–81.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16(5), 367–70.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kita, S. & Özyürek, A. (2003) What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language 48, 1632.Google Scholar
Kita, S. & Özyürek, A. (2007). How does spoken language shape iconic gestures? In Duncan, S., Cassell, J. & Levy, E. T. (eds), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language: essays in honor of David McNeill, 6774. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kunene, R. (2010). A comparative study of the development of multimodal narratives in French and Zulu children and adults (unpublished PhD dissertation). Université Stendhal, Grenoble.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: what gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (ed.) (2000). Language and gesture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Molinsky, A. L., Krabbenhoft, M. A., Ambady, N. & Choi, Y. S. (2005). Cracking the nonverbal code: intercultural competence and gesture recognition across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 36(3), 380–95.Google Scholar
Morris, D. (1994). Body talk: a world guide to gestures. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Özçaliskan, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture is at the cutting-edge of early language development. Cognition 96(3), 101–13.Google Scholar
Özçaliskan, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). When gesture–speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(2), 190217.Google Scholar
Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Allen, S., Brown, A., Furman, R. & Ishizuka, T. (2008). Development of cross-linguistic variation in speech and gesture: motion events in English and Turkish. Developmental Psychology 44(4), 1040–54.Google Scholar
Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Allen, S., Furman, R. & Brown, A. (2005). How does linguistic framing of events influence co-speech gestures? Insights from cross-linguistic variations and similarities. Gesture 5, 219–40.Google Scholar
Pika, S., Nicoladis, E. & Marentette, P. (2009). How to order a beer: cultural differences in the use of conventional gestures for numbers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 40(1), 7080.Google Scholar
Pizzuto, E. & Capobianco, M. (2005). The link and differences between deixis and symbols in children's early gestural–vocal system. Gesture 5(1/2), 179–99.Google Scholar
Reig Alamillo, A., Colletta, J.-M. & Guidetti, M. (2013). Gesture and language in narratives and explanations: the effects of age and communicative activity on late multimodal discourse development. Journal of Child Language 40(3), 511–38.Google Scholar
Sales Dias, M., Gibet, S., Wanderley, M. M. & Bastos, R. (eds) (2009). Gesture-based human–computer interaction and simulation: 7th International GestureWorkshop, GW 2007, Lisbon, Portugal, 23–25 May 2007, Revised selected papers (LNAI 5085). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Saville-Troike, M. (1982). The ethnography of communication: an introduction. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Simone, R. (2008). Fondamenti di Linguistica. Roma-Bari: Editori Laterza.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms. Language Typology and Syntactic Description 3, 57149.Google Scholar
Tolchinsky, L. (2004). The nature and scope of later language development. In Berman, R. A. (ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence, 233–47. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M. & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development 78(3), 705–22.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O. & Pizzuto, E. (2004). Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In Tomasello, M. & Slobin, D. (eds), Elisabeth Bates: a Festschrift, 340. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (1990). From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. Berlin/New York: Springer Verlag (1994: 2nd ed. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshioka, K. (2009). Gesture and information structure in first and second language. Gesture 8(2), 236–55.Google Scholar