Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Kalish, Charles
2005.
Becoming status conscious.
Philosophical Explorations,
Vol. 8,
Issue. 3,
p.
245.
Nayer, Samantha L.
and
Graham, Susan A.
2006.
Children's communicative strategies in novel and familiar word situations.
First Language,
Vol. 26,
Issue. 4,
p.
403.
Clark, Eve V.
2007.
Conventionality and contrast in language and language acquisition.
New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development,
Vol. 2007,
Issue. 115,
p.
11.
Moriguchi, Yusuke
Lee, Kang
and
Itakura, Shoji
2007.
Social transmission of disinhibition in young children.
Developmental Science,
Vol. 10,
Issue. 4,
p.
481.
Sabbagh, Mark A.
and
Henderson, Annette M. E.
2007.
How an appreciation of conventionality shapes early word learning.
New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development,
Vol. 2007,
Issue. 115,
p.
25.
Danovitch, Judith H.
and
Keil, Frank C.
2007.
Choosing between hearts and minds: Children's understanding of moral advisors.
Cognitive Development,
Vol. 22,
Issue. 1,
p.
110.
Koenig, Melissa A.
and
Harris, Paul L.
2007.
The Basis of Epistemic Trust: Reliable Testimony or Reliable Sources?.
Episteme,
Vol. 4,
Issue. 3,
p.
264.
Scofield, Jason
and
Behrend, Douglas A.
2008.
Learning words from reliable and unreliable speakers.
Cognitive Development,
Vol. 23,
Issue. 2,
p.
278.
Chambers, Craig G.
Graham, Susan A.
and
Turner, Juanita N.
2008.
When hearsay trumps evidence: How generic language guides preschoolers’ inferences about unfamiliar things.
Language and Cognitive Processes,
Vol. 23,
Issue. 5,
p.
749.
Sabbagh, Mark A.
and
Shafman, Dana
2009.
How children block learning from ignorant speakers.
Cognition,
Vol. 112,
Issue. 3,
p.
415.
Mascaro, Olivier
and
Sperber, Dan
2009.
The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children’s vigilance towards deception.
Cognition,
Vol. 112,
Issue. 3,
p.
367.
Diesendruck, Gil
Carmel, Nurit
and
Markson, Lori
2010.
Children’s Sensitivity to the Conventionality of Sources.
Child Development,
Vol. 81,
Issue. 2,
p.
652.
HENDERSON, ANNETTE M. E.
and
SABBAGH, MARK A.
2010.
Parents' use of conventional and unconventional labels in conversations with their preschoolers.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 37,
Issue. 4,
p.
793.
Krogh‐Jespersen, Sheila
and
Echols, Catharine H.
2012.
The Influence of Speaker Reliability on First Versus Second Label Learning.
Child Development,
Vol. 83,
Issue. 2,
p.
581.
AKHTAR, NAMEERA
MENJIVAR, JENNIFER
HOICKA, ELENA
and
SABBAGH, MARK A.
2012.
Learning foreign labels from a foreign speaker: the role of (limited) exposure to a second language.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 39,
Issue. 5,
p.
1135.
SOBEL, DAVID M.
SEDIVY, JULIE
BUCHANAN, DAVID W.
and
HENNESSY, RACHEL
2012.
Speaker reliability in preschoolers' inferences about the meanings of novel words.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 39,
Issue. 1,
p.
90.
Henderson, Annette M.E.
Sabbagh, Mark A.
and
Woodward, Amanda L.
2013.
Preschoolers’ selective learning is guided by the principle of relevance.
Cognition,
Vol. 126,
Issue. 2,
p.
246.
Wood, Lara A.
Kendal, Rachel L.
and
Flynn, Emma G.
2013.
Whom do children copy? Model-based biases in social learning.
Developmental Review,
Vol. 33,
Issue. 4,
p.
341.
Bascandziev, Igor
and
Harris, Paul L.
2014.
In beauty we trust: Children prefer information from more attractive informants.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Vol. 32,
Issue. 1,
p.
94.
Bascandziev, Igor
and
Harris, Paul L.
2016.
The beautiful and the accurate: Are children’s selective trust decisions biased?.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Vol. 152,
Issue. ,
p.
92.