Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T04:02:24.733Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Conversational asymmetry between mothers and children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Kenneth Kaye
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Rosalind Charney
Affiliation:
Cook County Hospital

Abstract

TURNABOUTS, which both respond to and require a response from the other (either verbal or nonverbal), were produced more than twice as often by mothers as by their children at 2; 2 and 2; 6, in videotaped dialogues during semi-structured play. The 27 mothers showed stable individual differences in this aspect of their turn-taking, across situations and across time. Children tended to take a reciprocal role rather than an imitative one within the immediate situation: if a mother produced many mands, her child produced few. At 2; 10 the children were tested for language comprehension, puzzle-solving and conversational engagement with an investigator. Mothers' mands at the earlier ages appeared to have a negative effect upon these measures, even when social class differences were controlled; but causal interpretations were complicated by the fact that language production measures also predicted the 2; 10 cognitive measures.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was funded by the Spencer Foundation. We are grateful to Patricia Benda, Solveig Dahlstrom, and Richard Pearse for transcribing and coding; and to Susan Goldin-Meadow for kindly reading and discussing the manuscript. Address for correspondence: Kenneth Kaye, University of Chicago, 5835 S. Kimbark Avenue, Chicago, Illinois 60637, U.S.A.

References

REFERENCES

Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult–child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. CogPsych 8. 521–52.Google Scholar
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B. & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: the early mother-infant interaction. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), The effects of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1968). The development of wh-questions in child speech. JVLVB 7. 279–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. (1977). The clarification request as a feature of adult interactive styles with young children. LangSoc 6. 183208.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. (1965). Peabody picture vocabulary test. Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1977). Some features of early child–adult dialogues. LangSoc 7. 357–73.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. (1973). Deixis II. Unpublished lectures. University of California, Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1977). The contingent query: a dependent act in conversation. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. & Berninger, G. (1981). Timing and turn-taking in children's conversations. Discourse processes (in press).Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1978). CRESCAT: Software system for analysis of sequential or real-time data. Chicago: University of Chicago Computation Center.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1980). Why we don't talk ‘Baby Talk’ to babies. JChLang 7. 489507.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1981). The mental and social life of babies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. & Charney, R. (1980). How mothers maintain ‘dialogue’ with two-year-olds. In Olson, D. (ed.), The social foundations of language and thought: essays in honor of Jerome S. Bruner. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. & Fogel, A. (1980). The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers and infants. DevPsych 16. 454–64.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. & Wells, A. J. (1980). Mothers' jiggling and the burst-pause pattern in neonatal feeding. InfantBehavDevel 3. 2946.Google Scholar
Martinez, M. (1980). Conversational asymmetry between Mexican mothers and children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Newson, J. (1977). An intersubjective approach to the systematic description of mother-infant interaction. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother—infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Richards, M. P. M. (1974). The development of psychological communication in the first year of life. In Connolly, K. J. & Bruner, J. S. (eds), The growth of competence. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Scarry, R. (1971). Richard Scarry's ABC word book. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. JChLang 4. 122.Google Scholar
Stern, D., Beebe, B., Jaffe, J. & Bennett, S. (1977). The infants' stimulus world during social interaction: a study of caregiver behaviors with particular reference to repetition and timing. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behavior. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar