Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T06:35:23.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The complexity of opportunities to respond used by mothers and fathers of children with Down syndrome: A preliminary investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 September 2024

Marianne Elmquist*
Affiliation:
Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI
Andrea L.B. Ford
Affiliation:
Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Cincinnati
Audra Sterling
Affiliation:
Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI
*
Corresponding author: Marianne Elmquist; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Caregiver-child interactions are commonly used to examine children’s language learning environment. However, few studies consider interaction configurations beyond dyadic interactions or explore the conceptual complexity of caregiver talk. Thus, we examined if the complexity of a caregiver’s opportunities to respond (OTR) varied when sampled across three interaction configurations. Our study included twelve preschool-aged children with Down syndrome and both of their biological parents. Our preliminary findings suggest no differences in mothers’ and fathers’ frequency of OTRs across complexity levels during dyadic interactions. However, caregivers produced fewer OTRs across complexity levels during family choice than dyadic interactions.

Type
Brief Research Report
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Down syndrome (DS) is the most common chromosomal cause of intellectual disability, with a prevalence of one in 691 live births (Parker et al., Reference Parker, Mai, Canfield, Rckard, Wang, Meyer, Anderson, Mason, Collins, Kirby and Correa2010). In addition to intellectual impairments, co-occurring language delays and impairments impact multiple aspects of language comprehension and production (Abbeduto et al., Reference Abbeduto, Warren and Conners2007; Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Price, Barnes, Nelson, Burchinal, Hennon, Moskowitz, Edwards, Malkin, Anderson, Misenheimer and Hooper2007). Moreover, these impairments present early in life for children with DS and persist throughout the lifespan. Given these language delays and impairments, early caregiver-child interactions are critical in providing the foundations for language learning for children with DS (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Hokstad and Næss2020; Woynaroski et al., Reference Woynaroski, Yoder, Fey and Warren2014).

Grounded in the transactional model of development (Sameroff, Reference Sameroff2010), there is a wealth of research in support of the substantial and cumulative impact of caregiver-child interactions on child language development both for neurotypical (NT) children and children with disabilities in Western countries (Bottema-Beutel et al., Reference Bottema-Beutel, Kim, Crowley and Yoder2021; Bozicevic et al., Reference Bozicevic, Hill, Chandra, Omirou, Holla, Wright and Sharp2023; Ford et al., Reference Ford, Elmquist, Merbler, Kriese, Will and McConnell2020; Pace et al., Reference Pace, Rojas, Bakeman, Adamson, Tamis-LeMonda, O’Brien Caughy, Owen and Suma2022; Woynaroski et al., Reference Woynaroski, Yoder, Fey and Warren2014). During these interactions, caregivers use various discrete language-facilitating strategies for different purposes that promote language learning and provide opportunities for children to demonstrate their communicative skills. These strategies include but are not limited to, expansions, follow-in comments, linguistic mapping, mirroring, modeling, and opportunities to respond (Elmquist et al., Reference Elmquist, Ford, Lorang and Sterling2024; Kaiser & Roberts, Reference Kaiser and Roberts2013; Mattie & Hadley, Reference Mattie and Hadley2021; McDuffie & Yoder, Reference McDuffie and Yoder2010). Given the importance of caregiver-child interactions, researchers and clinicians often examine (and intervene at) the structural features of caregiver talk (e.g., lexical diversity and complexity) to optimize language learning. Towards that premise, Rowe and Snow (Reference Rowe and Snow2019) discussed how caregiver talk can be analyzed along three dimensions that provide a framework for the current study. These dimensions are: (a) conceptual, such as conversational topics that provide a developmentally appropriate challenge (e.g., concrete/contextualized versus abstract/decontextualized), (b) interactional, such as features that support the child’s involvement (e.g., responsiveness, shared attention), and (c) linguistic or the phonological, lexical, and grammatical features of caregiver talk (Rowe & Snow, Reference Rowe and Snow2019). In this investigation, we narrowed our focus to two dimensions: 1) conceptual, 2) interactional. Specifically, we focused on the conceptual complexity of opportunities to respond (OTRS; Greenwood et al., Reference Greenwood, Delquadri, Hall, Herward, Heron and Trap-Porter1984). OTRs can be defined as caregiver communicative bids (e.g., questions, comments) that create openings for children to interact with their communication partners and, importantly, provide opportunities to demonstrate their communication and language skills (Ford et al., Reference Ford, Elmquist, Merbler, Kriese, Will and McConnell2020). We opted to focus on explicit OTRs (i.e., questions) given that asking questions compared to comments increase children’s response rates (Yoder & Davies, Reference Yoder and Davies1990; Yoder et al., Reference Yoder, Davies and Bishop1994). Children’s responses, in turn, provide additional opportunities for caregivers to continue the interaction.

Research examining the conceptual features of caregiver talk – and how it relates to child outcomes – among children with DS is limited. Most recently, Hilvert et al. (Reference Hilvert, Lorang and Sterling2021) found that mothers of children with DS tended to use the same proportion of decontextualized talk as mothers of NT children across three routines of free play, shared book reading, and snack, with the exception of pretend talk (e.g., the baby is hungry). They further found that mothers’ decontextualized talk was not associated with children’s chronological or developmental language ability. These same researchers also examined mothers’ and fathers’ decontextualized and contextualized talk during shared book reading among children with DS (Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang, Maltman and Sterling2022). Their results showed no differences in the type of talk between caregivers, except the fathers produced more reading utterances compared to mothers. Notably, they found that children’s expressive language and lexical diversity were associated with fathers’ use of decontextualized talk, not mothers. As a robust and vital facilitator of language learning, we sought to examine the extent to which caregiver OTRs were grounded in the here and now (i.e., contextualized talk) or involved more abstract and often more complex concepts that were not grounded in the here and now (i.e., decontextualized talk; Snow, Reference Snow1991) in caregiver-child interactions.

Interaction configuration and caregiver talk

Within the caregiver-child interaction literature, researchers have primarily measured dyadic interactions, with a significant portion focused solely on mother-child dyads (e.g., Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang and Sterling2021; Peredo et al., Reference Peredo, Dillehay and Kaiser2020). However, many children are likely to interact with multiple caregivers and siblings. Families composed of mother-father, two-caregiver households are just one example. Understanding how these interactions may change depending on the caregiver is critical, given that an emerging body of literature describes differences and similarities in mothers’ and fathers’ talk (e.g., Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang, Maltman and Sterling2022; Pancsofar & Vernon-Feagans, Reference Pancsofar and Vernon-Feagans2006). For example, mothers of children with DS provided more language and descriptive language than fathers during dyadic free play sessions (de Falco et al., Reference de Falco, Venuti, Esposito and Bornstein2011). These findings also extend to shared book reading: fathers spend more time reading the book text than mothers, who are more talkative and use more descriptive language (Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang, Maltman and Sterling2022). Furthermore, maternal and paternal language input was associated with different aspects of child language during book reading, suggesting that caregivers support language acquisition in divergent approaches (Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang and Sterling2021).

Different interaction configurations (e.g., dyadic versus triadic interactions) are also associated with differences in the nature and frequency of caregiver talk. For example, research including NT children has shown that during dyadic interactions, mothers’ and fathers’ words and utterances are more complex than their talk in triadic interactions (Nandy et al., Reference Nandy, Nixon and Quigley2021; Quigley & Nixon, Reference Quigley and Nixon2022). Elmquist et al. (Reference Elmquist, Ford, Lorang and Sterling2024) found that when combining mothers’ and fathers’ communicative bids, children with DS were exposed to fewer bids during interactions involving mothers, fathers, and siblings (if present) compared to dyadic caregiver-child interactions. Furthermore, there were differences in this effect based on the explicitness of the communicative bid (i.e., questions versus comments).

Caregiver-child interaction data collection methods

There are two general approaches to collecting caregiver-child interactions. The first is in-person observations that often involve an examiner being present and are typically collected at home or a research lab (e.g., Bottema-Beutel et al., Reference Bottema-Beutel, Malloy, Lloyd, Louick, Joffe-Nelson, Watson and Yoder2018; Potter et al., Reference Potter, Harvey, Sterling and Abbeduto2024). Standardized materials (e.g., toys, books) are often provided and caregivers are instructed to interact with their child as they typically would without an examiner present (e.g., Bottema-Beutel et al., Reference Bottema-Beutel, Malloy, Lloyd, Louick, Joffe-Nelson, Watson and Yoder2018). This approach often captures dyadic interactions, with mother-child dyads being the most common configuration studied. The second method involves long-form recordings without an examiner present (Casillas et al., Reference Casillas, Cristia, Zwaan and Dingemanse2019). These long-form recordings often utilize LENA recordings (e.g., Gilkerson et al., Reference Gilkerson, Richards, Warren, Montgomery, Greenwood, Kimbrough Oller, Hansen and Paul2017; Parikh & Mastergeorge, Reference Parikh and Mastergeorge2018) and caregivers are typically instructed to go about their typical routines but to remain at home (e.g., Thiemann-Bourque et al., Reference Thiemann-Bourque, Warren, Brady, Gilkerson and Richards2014). With these long-form recordings, there is opportunity to examine a variety of interaction configurations, such as mother-child, father-child, sibling-child, or even multiple caregivers/siblings and the focal child.

A comprehensive picture of the child’s communication interactions is a clear benefit of long-form recordings. Caregiver-implemented communication interventions are often taught and examined in dyadic configurations, but they are expected to generalize to interactions beyond these dyadic interactions. If we want to actualize this generalization instead of training and hoping (Stokes & Baer, Reference Stokes and Baer1977), we must examine caregivers’ and children’s communication in interactions that represent the configurations in which they will be implemented at the outset. In doing so, we can better understand language learning opportunities for children with DS and improve the generalized efficacy of future caregiver-implemented communication interventions.

Study purpose and research questions

To further our understanding of the language environments of children with DS, we must examine dimensions of language-facilitating strategies, such as the conceptual complexity of caregiver talk. However, to ensure we are comprehensive in our measurement, it is equally critical that we examine how language dimensions might change across different interaction configurations. This preliminary investigation examined if there were differences in the conceptual complexity of mothers’ and fathers’ use of one strategy – OTRs – across interaction configurations (e.g., dyadic free play video observations vs. family choice LENA observations). The current research questions (RQ) guided the present study:

  • RQ1: Are there differences in the conceptual complexity of OTRs between mothers and fathers during dyadic caregiver-child interactions?

  • RQ2: Do mothers’ and fathers’ produce fewer conceptually complex OTRs in family choice interactions compared to dyadic caregiver-child interactions?

  • RQ3: Are there differences in the conceptual complexity of OTRs children are exposed to during dyadic versus family choice interactions when mothers’ and fathers’ OTRs are combined?

Methods

Participants

Twelve children with DS (M = 3 years; 4 months, SD = 12.16 months) and their biological parents participated in the current study. This age range was selected because during the preschool years children’s language becomes more complex and parent’s use of decontextualized talk increases (e.g., Rowe, Reference Rowe2012). Participants for the present study came from an existing dataset (Elmquist et al., Reference Elmquist, Ford, Lorang and Sterling2024) and included participants from a larger, previously published dataset (n = 15; see Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang, Maltman and Sterling2022; Lorang et al., Reference Lorang, Hartley and Sterling2020; Lorang & Sterling, Reference Lorang and Sterling2021). We included families in the current study if they had completed the following caregiver-child interactions: dyadic mother-child freeplay, dyadic father-child freeplay, and family choice LENA interactions. Caregivers provided genetic documentation of trisomy 21. Children lived in two-caregiver households and had normal or corrected hearing and vision. All families were white, non-Hispanic/Latino, and monolingual English-speaking. Families were recruited from clinics, centers, and early intervention providers serving families with DS in Madison, Wisconsin, USA, and the surrounding areas. We used the Receptive and Expressive subscales of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, Reference Mullen1995), to characterize children’s language abilities. A trained examiner administered the MSEL during one of the two home visits. Table 1 provides complete demographic information.

Table 1. Participant Characteristics (n = 12)

Note. Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, Reference Mullen1995). SALT = Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Andriacchi and Nockerts2011). aDerived from dyadic father-child interaction.

Procedures

The University’s Institutional Review Board approved the study, and written informed consent was obtained from both parents. All examiner-led study procedures were completed in the family’s home over two visits.

Caregiver-child interactions

We employed two commonly used methods to collect caregiver-child interactions for the current study: video observations collected in the home for dyadic caregiver-child interactions (e.g., Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang, Maltman and Sterling2022; Leezenbaum et al., Reference Leezenbaum, Campbell, Butler and Iverson2014) and a LENA recording for the family choice context (e.g., Parikh & Mastergeorge, Reference Parikh and Mastergeorge2018; Thiemann-Bourque et al., Reference Thiemann-Bourque, Warren, Brady, Gilkerson and Richards2014). Ten-minute dyadic interactions were video-recorded separately for mother-child and father-child interactions during free-play observations with an examiner present. Families were provided with a developmentally appropriate standardized toy set, and caregivers were instructed to “play as you typically would” but to only play with the included items (see supplemental material for a list of toys used). The order of dyadic interactions was randomized across participants. We used LENA recorders to collect family choice interactions without an examiner present. This method was used to capture more naturalistic family interactions. Families completed a 3-hour LENA recording during an evening, and this was completed before the first examiner visit. Each family was given instructions, a LENA recorder, and specialized clothing to hold the LENA. Caregivers were instructed to go about their everyday family routines (e.g., play, mealtimes, laundry). Both caregivers were present; in five instances, siblings were also present.

Transcription and coding

Trained research assistants from a communication disorders program completed all transcriptions and coding. Transcribers and coders completed a standardized training protocol using caregiver-child interactions not included in the current dataset. There was some overlap between coders and transcribers, but all were blinded to the study research questions. Coders started independent work after completing three consecutive training transcripts at or above 80% reliability across all OTR and transcription measures.

All interactions were transcribed using the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) conventions and software (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Andriacchi and Nockerts2011). While LENA recordings provide automated measures of adult and child vocal behaviors, we transcribed a portion of the recording, adapting procedures from Wood et al. (Reference Wood, Diehm and Callender2016). Specifically, a bar graph of vocalizations of the 3-hour recording was visually inspected to identify a high-volubility 10-minute segment (i.e., a segment containing the greatest amount of caregiver and child talk). This segment was then transcribed using SALT conventions. Some dyadic recordings were shorter than the planned 10-minutes; to ensure consistency across participants and conditions, we only used the first 8-minutes (the longest time length for one participant) of all interactions for analyses.

Coded variables

In this study, we defined OTRs as caregiver utterances that provided an explicit response pathway for a child (i.e., questions). These had been previously coded in Elmquist et al. (Reference Elmquist, Ford, Lorang and Sterling2024). For the current project, we coded each OTR for its conceptual complexity using codes adapted from Massey et al. (Reference Massey, Pence, Justice and Bowles2008). We had three mutually exclusive codes: management, less conceptually challenging (LLC), and more conceptually challenging (MCC). Of note, OTRs that were coded as MCC are also likely to include more complex syntax compared to management or LCC OTRs. See Table 2 for operational definitions that include examples of what was coded for each code and what was not. Our full coding manual is included in supplemental materials.

Table 2. Definitions and Examples for Coding Conceptual Complexity of Caregiver OTRs

Reliability

We obtained reliability data for the transcription and coding process. For transcriptions, 24% of interactions were transcribed by a second transcriber. Line-by-line percent agreement was calculated for segmentation, number of morphemes, number of words, word identification, and intelligibility (M = 88.75%, min-max: 85.92-94.48%). For OTR complexity coding, 28% of interactions were initially coded by a second coder. Reliability was below 80% (M = 57%, min-max: 8-92%) due to the low base rates of the MCC code. As a result, all explicit OTRs were consensus coded for complexity codes.

Data analysis

Given our small sample size, multiple comparisons, and dependencies between comparisons, we calculated Hedges g and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) to examine differences in caregiver OTRs across interaction configurations (Nakagawa & Cuthill, Reference Nakagawa and Cuthill2007; Patriota, Reference Patriota2017). We interpreted effect sizes as small (g = .20), medium (g = .50), and large (g = .80; Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). Specifically, we compared differences in mothers’ and fathers’ complexity of OTRs between mother-child and father-child dyadic interactions and within caregiver differences across dyadic and family choice interactions (e.g., mother dyadic versus mother family choice). For our third research question, we were interested in how interaction configurations impacted the complexity of OTRs directed to the child. Therefore, we compared the OTRs produced by mothers and fathers in their respective dyadic interactions with the combined OTRs of mothers and fathers in family choice interactions.

Results

Table 3 displays means, standard deviations, and ranges for the conceptual complexity of caregivers’ OTRs across interaction configurations. Across caregivers and interaction configurations, more OTRs were classified as management (mean range: 12.46-54.58), followed by less conceptually challenging (mean range: 1.5-16.50-) and more conceptually challenging (mean range: 0.08-2.08-). Given the large observed standard deviations across study measures – suggesting inter-caregiver variability – we also examined individual caregiver data to understand how caregivers may have contributed to group results (see Figure 1). Table 4 displays the results for Hedges g and 95% confidence intervals.

Table 3. Descriptive Statistics for Caregiver OTR Complexity

Note. LCC = Less conceptually challenging. MCC = more conceptually challenging

Figure 1. Number of OTRs Across Complexity Levels and Interaction Configuration

Note. Dy-F = father OTRs during dyadic father-child interaction. Dy-M = mother OTRs during dyadic mother-child interaction. CH-B = mother and father OTRs combined during family choice interaction. Ch-F = father OTRs during family choice interaction. Ch-M = mother OTRs during family choice interaction. LLC = less conceptually challenging. MCC = more conceptually challenging.

Table 4. Hedges g and 95% Confidence Intervals for Caregiver OTR Complexity

Note. LCC = Less conceptually challenging. MCC = more conceptually challenging.

RQ1: Mother versus father dyadic comparisons

We observed medium effects of fathers producing fewer management OTRs than mothers during dyadic caregiver-child interactions. This finding was the same for less conceptually challenging OTRs. In contrast, we observed a small effect for fathers producing more conceptually challenging OTRs than mothers. However, confidence intervals contained zero for all results, indicating that these effects were not statistically significant had we calculated p-values.

RQ2: Within caregiver differences between dyadic and family choice interactions

Mothers and fathers produced fewer OTRs across complexity classification during family choice interactions than their respective dyadic caregiver-child interactions. We observed large effects for management and less conceptually challenging OTRs. For more conceptually challenging OTRs, we observed large effects for fathers and negligible effects for mothers. For all analyses, except for the mother’s more conceptually challenging OTRs, confidence intervals did not contain zero, indicating differences are likely to be meaningful.

RQ3: Combined caregiver OTRs in family choice compared to dyadic interactions

We observed large effects of children being exposed to fewer management OTRs when combining mothers’ and fathers’ management OTRs compared to their respective dyadic interactions. Similarly, we observed large effects for less conceptually challenging OTRs. Small effects were observed when comparing combined family choice with the father’s dyadic use of more conceptually challenging OTRs and negligible effects were observed for mothers. For all results but the mother’s use of management and less conceptually challenging OTRs, confidence intervals contained zero, suggesting differences observed are not meaningful.

Discussion

Caregiver-child interactions are essential, robust, and facilitative contexts for promoting language learning in young children with DS. Furthermore, research suggests that mothers and fathers use different approaches to support language development (e.g., de Falco et al., Reference de Falco, Venuti, Esposito and Bornstein2011; Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang, Maltman and Sterling2022; Pancsofar & Vernon-Feagans, Reference Pancsofar and Vernon-Feagans2006). However, a paucity of studies have examined the conceptual complexity of mothers’ and fathers’ utterances across interaction configurations to which young children with DS are exposed. We aimed to understand if there were conceptual complexity differences in mothers’ and fathers’ OTRs across interaction configurations.

For our first research question, we compared mothers’ and fathers’ OTR complexity during dyadic caregiver-child interactions. Effect sizes indicated that mothers produced more management and less conceptually challenging OTRs but fewer more conceptually challenging OTRs than fathers. These effects, however, are unlikely to be meaningful given our confidence intervals. As such, we have preliminary evidence that there may be no differences among mothers’ and fathers’ use of a range of conceptually complex OTRs during dyadic free-play interactions. These results align with the findings of Hilvert et al. (Reference Hilvert, Lorang, Maltman and Sterling2022), who similarly found no differences amongst caregivers across most types of talk during shared book reading – however, there was overlap between the two samples. While more exploration and replication is needed, we have initial evidence that caregivers’ style of talk may be consistent across dyadic interaction contexts. If this finding is replicated, ideally with larger more diverse samples, it will likely have implications for intervention and generalization.

We found that while fathers produced a higher frequency of more conceptually challenging OTRs compared to mothers during dyadic interactions, these results are unlikely to be meaningful in our study. This finding adds to the conflicting evidence in the literature: some studies showing differences in fathers and mothers decontextualized talk (i.e., talk not grounded in the here and now; e.g., Duursma, Reference Duursma2016) while others show no difference (e.g., Hilvert et al., Reference Hilvert, Lorang, Maltman and Sterling2022). More research is needed to explore if meaningful differences exist between mothers and fathers in the conceptual complexity of their talk, and if so the implication of those differences.

The present study also examined caregiver differences across interaction configurations (i.e., RQ2). We found large and meaningful effects for mothers and fathers producing fewer management and less conceptually challenging OTRs during family choice interactions than their respective dyadic interactions. We also found significant and meaningful effects for fathers producing fewer more conceptually challenging OTRs during family choice interactions than father-child interactions. In contrast, negligible effects were observed for mothers. This is unsurprising given that mothers did not use many of the more conceptually challenging OTRs regardless of interaction configuration. Our findings from our second research question align with the limited NT research examining dyadic and triadic caregiver-child interactions. Nandy et al. (Reference Nandy, Nixon and Quigley2021) also found that parents produced fewer utterances during triadic interactions than their dyadic interactions. We would also expect interactions involving more communication partners (e.g., multiple caregivers and siblings) to result in fewer opportunities for one-on-one caregiver-child interactions (McHale & Fivaz-Depeursinge, Reference McHale and Fivaz-Depeursinge1999).

Lastly, we examined if children with DS were exposed to differing amounts of OTRs across complexity levels during dyadic and family choice interactions (i.e., RQ3). We found that children were exposed to fewer OTRs across complexity levels during more naturalistic family contexts (i.e., family choice), compared to dyadic interactions, even when combining caregiver OTRs. However, our results suggest that these differences are not meaningful for most comparisons, except for mothers’ use of management and less conceptually challenging OTRs. These findings diverge from Nandy et al. (Reference Nandy, Nixon and Quigley2021), who found that children during triadic interactions were exposed to more caregiver utterances when combining mothers’ and fathers’ utterances than dyadic interactions. This difference might be due to child characteristics (e.g., DS versus NT), different approaches to measuring caregiver talk, the context of interactions, or because some of our family choice interactions also included siblings (n = 5).

Overall, our findings present preliminary evidence that interaction configuration has a greater impact on differences in caregiver talk than communication partner (i.e., mother versus father). These findings have important implications for caregiver-mediated communication interventions. These interventions are often taught and examined in dyadic contexts yet are intended to be implemented in contexts that extend dyadic interactions. Our results suggest that the dosage of these interventions may be influenced by interaction configuration. More research is needed to understand how interaction configurations influence child language learning among children with DS and the impact on intervention implementation and efficacy.

Limitations and future research

The present study had several limitations, which should be considered when interpreting the results. First, the small sample size included children with a relatively wide age range and considerable caregiver talk variability. As a result, we had large CIs, which impacted the precision of our effect sizes and reduced our findings’ generalizability. This is particularly important given the known heterogeneity in language profiles observed among children with DS. Second, our study comprised white, mono-English, heterosexual caregivers. To increase the generalizability of future research, research that is more inclusive of individuals from marginalized backgrounds is needed. Third, we cannot determine if the recording mode of caregiver-child interactions influenced differences in our results. Future research must examine whether the method used to collect caregiver-child interactions influences our measurement of caregiver and child talk. Lastly, we need longitudinal research across interaction configurations to test and identify caregiver talk associated with language learning that can be leveraged in future caregiver-mediated communication interventions.

Conclusions

Our preliminary evidence suggests that mothers and fathers did not differ in using OTRs across complexity levels during dyadic caregiver-child interactions. In contrast, interaction configuration did influence caregivers’ frequency of OTRs across complexity levels, with caregivers producing fewer OTRs during family choice interactions compared to dyadic interactions. While children with DS are likely to be exposed to fewer OTRs across complexity levels during family choice interactions, these differences may not be meaningful depending on the type of OTR. While these findings need to be replicated, our results suggest that interaction configurations need to be carefully considered in the development of caregiver implemented communication interventions. Additionally, more descriptive research – concurrent and longitudinal – is needed to gather a holistic understanding of how interaction configurations impact aspects of caregiver talk beyond conceptual complexity.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000370.

Acknowledgements

The authors want to acknowledge their positionality related to the current study: all authors are early child language researchers and identify as white, and two authors also identify as early intervention researchers. All authors are parents. Two of the three are parents of neurotypical children, and the third is a parent of both neurotypical and neurodivergent children. This study was supported in part by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number T32HD007489 and P50HD105353 awarded to the University of Wisconsin-Madison, as well as a Vilas life cycle award, and startup funds from the University of Wisconsin-Madison. The content is solely the authors’ responsibility and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The authors thank the families that took time out of their busy lives to participate in the current study. Additionally, the authors would like to thank Susen Schroeder, Kelsey Reis, Melissa Hauptman, Shelby Gunderson, Rebecca Willer, Kallie Renfus, Holly Erbstoesser from the Research in Developmental Disabilities and Language Lab who assisted with transcription and coding efforts.

Competing interest declaration

The authors declare “none”.

References

Abbeduto, L., Warren, S. F., & Conners, F. A. (2007). Language development in Down syndrome: From the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13(3), 247261. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bottema-Beutel, K., Kim, S. Y., Crowley, S., & Yoder, P. J. (2021). Developmental associations between joint engagement and autistic children’s vocabulary: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Autism, 25(2), 566575. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320968641CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottema-Beutel, K., Malloy, C., Lloyd, B. P., Louick, R., Joffe-Nelson, L., Watson, L. R., & Yoder, P. J. (2018). Sequential Associations Between Caregiver Talk and Child Play in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Typical Development. Child Development, 89(3), e157e166. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12848CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozicevic, L., Hill, J., Chandra, P. S., Omirou, A., Holla, C., Wright, N., & Sharp, H. (2023). Cross-cultural differences in early caregiving: Levels of mind-mindedness and instruction in UK and India. Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/frcha.2023.1124883CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casillas, M., Cristia, A., Zwaan, R., & Dingemanse, M. (2019). A step-by-step guide to collecting and analyzing long-format speech environment (LFSE) recordings. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1). https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.209Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Set Correlation and Contingency Tables. Applied Psychological Measurement, 12(4), 425434. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662168801200410CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Falco, S., Venuti, P., Esposito, G., & Bornstein, M. H. (2011). Maternal and paternal pragmatic speech directed to young children with Down syndrome and typical development. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(1), 161169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duursma, E. (2016). Who does the reading, who the talking? Low-income fathers and mothers in the US interacting with their young children around a picture book. First Language, 36(5), 465484. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723716648849CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elmquist, M., Ford, A. L. B., Lorang, E., & Sterling, A. (2024). Opportunities to Respond During Dyadic Caregiver–Child and Naturalistic Family Interactions Among Children With Down Syndrome: A Preliminary Investigation. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. https://doi.org/10.1044/2024_AJSLP-23-00179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, A. L. B., Elmquist, M., Merbler, A. M., Kriese, A., Will, K. K., & McConnell, S. R. (2020). Toward an ecobehavioral model of early language development. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Warren, S. F., Montgomery, J. K., Greenwood, C. R., Kimbrough Oller, D., Hansen, J. H. L., & Paul, T. D. (2017). Mapping the Early Language Environment Using All-Day Recordings and Automated Analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 26(2), 248265. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_AJSLP-15-0169CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwood, C. R., Delquadri, J., & Hall, R. V. (1984). Opportunity to respond and student academic performance. In Herward, W., Heron, T., & Trap-Porter, J. (Eds.), Behavior analysis in education (pp. 5888). Charles E. Merrill.Google Scholar
Hilvert, E., Lorang, E., Maltman, N., & Sterling, A. (2022). Assessing the Quantity and Quality of Language Used by Mothers and Fathers of Children with Down Syndrome During Shared Book Reading. Journal of Child Language, 113. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000922000046Google ScholarPubMed
Hilvert, E., Lorang, E., & Sterling, A. (2021). Maternal Use of Decontextualized and Contextualized Talk: An In-Depth Investigation of Early Parent-Child Interactions in Down Syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 30(4), 17671780. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_AJSLP-20-00190CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, A. P., & Roberts, M. Y. (2013). Parents as Communication Partners: An Evidence-Based Strategy for Improving Parent Support for Language and Communication in Everyday Settings. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education, 20(3), 96111. https://doi.org/10.1044/lle20.3.96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leezenbaum, N. B., Campbell, S. B., Butler, D., & Iverson, J. M. (2014). Maternal verbal responses to communication of infants at low and heightened risk of autism. Autism, 18(6), 694703. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361313491327CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorang, E., Hartley, S., & Sterling, A. (2020). Physiological arousal and observed behaviour in parent–child interactions involving young children with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 64(6), 426433. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12714CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorang, E., & Sterling, A. (2021). Parent Command Use and Child Compliance During Parent–Child Interactions Including Children With Down Syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 30(3), 12031209. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_AJSLP-20-00251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massey, S. L., Pence, K. L., Justice, L. M., & Bowles, R. P. (2008). Educators’ Use of Cognitively Challenging Questions in Economically Disadvantaged Preschool Classroom Contexts. Early Education & Development, 19(2), 340360. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280801964119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattie, L. J., & Hadley, P. A. (2021). Characterizing the Richness of Maternal Input for Word Learning in Neurogenetic Disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language, 42(4), 301317. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0041-1730914Google ScholarPubMed
McDuffie, A., & Yoder, P. (2010). Types of parent verbal responsiveness that predict language in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 53, 10261039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McHale, J. P., & Fivaz-Depeursinge, E. (1999). Understanding triadic and family group interactions during infancy and toddlerhood. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2, 107127. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021847714749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. F., Andriacchi, K., & Nockerts, A. (2011). Assessing language production using SALT software: A clinician’s guide to language sample analysis. SALT Software, LLC.Google Scholar
Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales for early Learning. AGS.Google Scholar
Nakagawa, S., & Cuthill, I. C. (2007). Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: A practical guide for biologists. Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 82(4), 591605. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00027.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nandy, A., Nixon, E., & Quigley, J. (2021). Communicative functions of parents’ child-directed speech across dyadic and triadic contexts. Journal of Child Language, 48(6), 12811294. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500092000080XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pace, A., Rojas, R., Bakeman, R., Adamson, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., O’Brien Caughy, M., Owen, M. T., and Suma, K. (2022). A Longitudinal Study of Language Use During Early Mother–Child Interactions in Spanish-Speaking Families Experiencing Low Income. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 65(1), 303319. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00329CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young children: Contributions to later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(6), 571587. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parikh, C., & Mastergeorge, A. M. (2018). Vocalization patterns in young children with Down syndrome: Utilizing the language environment analysis (LENA) to inform behavioral phenotypes. Journal of Intellectual Disability, 22(4), 328345. https://doi.org/10.1177/1744629517708091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, S. E., Mai, C. T., Canfield, M. A., Rckard, R., Wang, Y., Meyer, R. E., Anderson, P., Mason, C. A., Collins, J. S., Kirby, R. S., Correa, A., and Network for the N. B. D. P. (2010). Updated national birth prevalence estimates for selected birth defects in the United States, 2004–2006. Birth Defects Research Part A: Clinical and Molecular Teratology, 88(12), 10081016. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdra.20735CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patriota, A. G. (2017). On Some Assumptions of the Null Hypothesis Statistical Testing. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 77(3), 507528. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164416667979CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peredo, T. N., Dillehay, K. M., & Kaiser, A. P. (2020). Latino Caregivers’ Interactions With Their Children With Language Delays: A Comparison Study. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271121419900269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, S. N., Harvey, D., Sterling, A., & Abbeduto, L. (2024). Parental Responsivity and Child Communication During Mother–Child and Father–Child Interactions in Fragile X Syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 67(3), 939959. https://doi.org/10.1044/2023_JSLHR-23-00517CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quigley, J., & Nixon, E. (2022). Parent child directed speech in dyadic and triadic interaction: Associations with co-parenting dynamics and child language outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 58, 125135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J., Price, J., Barnes, E., Nelson, L., Burchinal, M., Hennon, E. A., Moskowitz, L., Edwards, A., Malkin, C., Anderson, K., Misenheimer, J., & Hooper, S. R. (2007). Receptive Vocabulary, Expressive Vocabulary, and Speech Production of Boys With Fragile X Syndrome in Comparison to Boys With Down Syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112(3), 177193. https://doi.org/10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[177:RVEVAS]2.0.CO;2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 17621774. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L., & Snow, C. E. (2019). Analyzing input quality along three dimensions: Interactive, linguistic, and conceptual. Journal of Child Language, 117. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000655Google ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Development, 81(1), 622. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01378.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E., Hokstad, S., & Næss, K.-A. B. (2020). Children with Down syndrome can benefit from language interventions; Results from a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 85, 105992. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2020.105992CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1991). The Theoretical Basis For Relationships Between Language and Literacy in Development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6(1), 510. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568549109594817CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An Implicit Technology of Generalization1. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10(2), 349367. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1977.10-349CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thiemann-Bourque, K. S., Warren, S. F., Brady, N., Gilkerson, J., & Richards, J. A. (2014). Vocal interaction between children with Down syndrome and their parents. .American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(3), 474485. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_AJSLP-12-0010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, C., Diehm, E. A., & Callender, M. F. (2016). An Investigation of Language Environment Analysis Measures for Spanish-English Bilingual Preschoolers From Migrant Low-Socioeconomic-Status Backgrounds. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 47(2), 123134. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_LSHSS-14-0115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woynaroski, T., Yoder, P. J., Fey, M. E., & Warren, S. F. (2014). A transactional model of spoken vocabulary variation in toddlers with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 57(5), 17541763. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0252CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoder, P. J., & Davies, B. (1990). Do parental questions and topic continuations elecit replies from developmentally delayed children?: A sequentional analysis. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 33, 563573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoder, P. J., Davies, B., & Bishop, K. (1994). Reciprocal sequential relations in conversations between parents and children with developmental delays. Journal of Early Intervention, 18(3), 362379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant Characteristics (n = 12)

Figure 1

Table 2. Definitions and Examples for Coding Conceptual Complexity of Caregiver OTRs

Figure 2

Table 3. Descriptive Statistics for Caregiver OTR Complexity

Figure 3

Figure 1. Number of OTRs Across Complexity Levels and Interaction ConfigurationNote. Dy-F = father OTRs during dyadic father-child interaction. Dy-M = mother OTRs during dyadic mother-child interaction. CH-B = mother and father OTRs combined during family choice interaction. Ch-F = father OTRs during family choice interaction. Ch-M = mother OTRs during family choice interaction. LLC = less conceptually challenging. MCC = more conceptually challenging.

Figure 4

Table 4. Hedges g and 95% Confidence Intervals for Caregiver OTR Complexity

Supplementary material: File

Elmquist et al. supplementary material 1

Elmquist et al. supplementary material
Download Elmquist et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 14 KB
Supplementary material: File

Elmquist et al. supplementary material 2

Elmquist et al. supplementary material
Download Elmquist et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 23.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Elmquist et al. supplementary material 3

Elmquist et al. supplementary material
Download Elmquist et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 47.2 KB