Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:11:32.691Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Competition between word order and case-marking in interpreting grammatical relations: a case study in multilingual acquisition*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 November 2010

CARMEL O'SHANNESSY*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, University of Michigan
*
Address for correspondence: e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The study examines strategies multilingual children use to interpret grammatical relations, focusing on their two primary languages, Lajamanu Warlpiri and Light Warlpiri. Both languages use mixed systems for indicating grammatical relations. In both languages ergative–absolutive case-marking indicates core arguments, but to different extents in each language. In Lajamanu Warlpiri, pronominal clitics in a nominative–accusative pattern also indicate core arguments, and in Light Warlpiri word order in a nominative–accusative pattern partially does so. The study asks which sentence interpretation strategies children rely on most, when they learn to rely on them and whether cross-linguistic influences are seen. Children aged 5 ; 0, 7 ; 0 and 9 ; 0 and adults saw paired, animated events simultaneously on video and heard a transitive sentence spoken. The participants pointed to the event depicted by the sentence heard. Adults used a case-marking strategy consistently in both languages. Children initially used both case-marking and word order strategies, but used case-marking more often as age increased.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

My sincere thanks go to members of Lajamanu Community, especially the participants in the study. I am very grateful to Melissa Bowerman, Penelope Brown, Bhuvana Narasimhan and Jane Simpson for assistance developing the study, to Harald Baayen for the statistical analysis, Jidong Chen for help with stimulus preparation, Seza Doğruoz for the Turkish examples, and to the Journal editors and anonymous reviewers for their comments. The article is an amended version of sections of my PhD thesis ‘Language contact and children's bilingual acquisition: learning a mixed language and Warlpiri in northern Australia.’

References

REFERENCES

Aksu-Koç, A. (1985). The acquisition of Turkish. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 1, 839–78. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Austin, J. (2007). Grammatical interference and the acquisition of ergative case in bilingual children learning Basque and Spanish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 10(3), 315–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavin, E. L. (1992). The acquisition of Warlpiri. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 3, 309372. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bavin, E. L. & Shopen, T. (1985). Children's acquisition of Warlpiri: Comprehension of transitive sentences. Journal of Child Language 12, 597610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavin, E. L. & Shopen, T. (1989). Cues to sentence interpretation in Warlpiri. In McWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (eds), The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing, 185205. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1981). Language development. In Triandis, H. C. & Heron, A. (eds), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Developmental psychology, Vol. 4, 93–185. Boston/London/Sydney/Toronto: Allyn and Bacon, Inc.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 12, 335–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Houwer, A. (1990). The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W. (1979). Ergativity. Language 55(1), 59–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Döpke, S. (2000). Generation of and retraction from cross-linguistically motivated structures in bilingual first language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3(3), 209–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E. & Paradis, J. (1995). Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language 22, 611–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Givon, T. (1988). The pragmatics of word-order: Predictability, importance and attention. In Hammond, M., Moravcsik, E. & Wirth, J. (eds), Studies in syntactic typology, 243–84. Amsterdam/Philadephia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, H. (2003). Multilevel statistical models. London: Hodder Arnold.Google Scholar
Hale, K. (1982). Some essential features of Warlpiri verbal clauses. In Swartz, S. (ed.), Papers in Warlpiri grammar: In memory of Lother Jagst, 217314. Darwin: Summer Institute of Linguistics-Australian Aborigines Branch.Google Scholar
Hale, K. (1992). Basic word order in two ‘free word order’ languages. In Payne, D. (ed.), Pragmatics of word order flexibility, 6382. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, K., Laughren, M. & Simpson, J. (1995). Warlpiri. In Jacobs, J., von Stechow, A., Sternefeld, W. & Vennemann, T. (eds), An international handbook of contemporary research, Vol. 2, 1430–49. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jelinek, E. (1984). Empty categories, case and configurationality. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 2(1), 39–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laughren, M. (2002). Syntactic constraints in a ‘free word order’ language. In Amberber, M. & Collins, P. (eds), Language universals and variation, 83–130. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1987). Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (1978). Sentential devices for conveying givenness and newness: A cross-cultural developmental study. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 17, 539–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matras, Y. & Bakker, P. (2003). The study of mixed languages. In Matras, Y. & Bakker, P. (eds), The mixed language debate, 120. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, W. (2010). Optional ergative case-marking systems in a typological perspective. Lingua 20(7), 1610–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meakins, F. & O'Shannessy, C. (2010). Ordering arguments about: Word order and discourse motivations in the development and use of the ergative marker in two Australian mixed languages. Lingua 20(7), 1693–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (1986). Word order and case marking in early child language. Evidence from simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: French and German. Linguistics 24, 123–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithun, M. (1987). Is basic word order universal? In Payne, D. L. (ed.), Pragmatics of word order flexibility, 1561. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Müller, N. & Hulk, A. (2001). Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 4(1), 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Shannessy, C. (2005). Light Warlpiri – a new language. Australian Journal of Linguistics 25(1), 3157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Shannessy, C. (2009). Language variation and change in a north Australian Indigenous community. In Preston, D. & Stanford, J. (eds), Variationist approaches to indigenous minority languages, 419–39. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E. (1985). Variation and error: A sociolinguistic approach to language acquisition in Samoa. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), Crosslinguistic studies in language acquisition, Vol. 1, 783838. London: Lawrence Erlbam Associates.Google Scholar
Pinheiro, J. & Bates, D. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S Plus. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pye, C. (1990). The acquisition of ergative languages. Linguistics 28, 1291–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raaijmakers, J., Schrijnemakers, J. & Gremmen, F. (1999). How to deal with ‘The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy’: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Journal of Memory and Language 41, 416–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, D. (1988). Variable rules. In Ammon, U., Dittmar, N. & Mettheier, K. J. (eds), Sociolinguistics: An international handbook of the science of language and society, 984–97. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Schieffelin, B. (1985). The acquisition of Kaluli. In Slobin, D. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: The data, Vol. 1, 525–93. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Simpson, J. (2007). Expressing pragmatic constraints on word order in Warlpiri. In Grimshaw, J., Maling, J., Manning, C., Simpson, J. & Zaenen, A. (eds), Architecture, rules and preferences: A festschrift for Joan Bresnan, 403427. Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information Publications.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1982). Universal and particular in acquisition. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: The state of the art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. & Bever, T. (1982). Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition 12, 229–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swartz, S. (1991). Constraints on zero anaphora and word order in Warlpiri narrative text. Darwin: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Van Valin, R. D. (1992). An overview of ergative phenomena and their implications for language acquisition. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 3, 1537. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar