Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T06:06:37.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Clause-initial AND usage in a cross-sectional and longitudinal corpus of school-age children's narratives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 April 2020

Jeffrey E. KALLAY*
Affiliation:
University of Oregon
Melissa A. REDFORD
Affiliation:
University of Oregon
*
*Corresponding author: Linguistics Department, 1290 University of Oregon, Eugene, OR97403 E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Young children adopt an event-chaining strategy when storytelling, frequently linking clauses with and. The current study tested whether age-related changes in clause-initial and usage might index narrative structure development in the Eugene Children's Story Corpus (ECSC), which includes 180 structured spontaneous narratives elicited yearly for three years from 60 children, aged five to seven at study onset. The narratives were segmented into clauses to quantify clause-initial and usage. Adult judgments of narrative coherence and cohesiveness were elicited as measures of narrative structure. Mean length of utterance (MLU) and clause (MLC) were used as measures of language complexity. Results indicated developmental increases in all measures, but only and-connected dependent clause usage increased with cross-sectional and longitudinal age. Only MLC predicted the relative frequency of clause-initial and regardless of children's age. These results suggest children's frequent use of and to connect events reflects immature language; its association with flat narrative structure is likely epiphenomenal.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acheson, D.J., & MacDonald, M.C. (2009). Verbal working memory and language production: common approaches to the serial ordering of verbal information. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 5068. doi: 10.1037/a0014411CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, A, & Gathercole, S.E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38, 403–14. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3802.403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, A., & Gathercole, S.E. (2000). Limitations in working memory: implications for language development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 95116. doi: 10.1080/136828200247278Google ScholarPubMed
Berman, R.A. (1997). Typological perspectives on connectivity. In Dittmar, N. & Penner, Z. (eds.), Issues in the theory of language acquisition (pp. 203–24). Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Berman, R.A., & Slobin, D.I. (Eds.). (1994). Relating events in narrative: a crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Biber, D. (1993). Representativeness in corpora design. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 8(4), 243–57. doi: 10.1093/llc/8.4.243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Lahey, M., Hood, L., Lifter, K., & Fiess, K. (1980). Complex sentences: acquisition of syntactic connectives and the semantic relations they encode. Journal of Child Language, 7, 235–61. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900002610CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvin, G.J., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1977). The development of structural complexity in children's fantasy narratives. Developmental Psychology, 13(4), 377–88. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.13.4.377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1979). The acquisition of complex sentences. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition (pp. 285305). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W.L. (2008). Aspects of discourse analysis. Brno studies in English, 34, 2337.Google Scholar
Connor, L.A. (2012). Factors affecting the development of character referentiality in preschoolers’ narratives (Doctoral dissertation). Theses and dissertations. Lehigh Preserve.Google Scholar
Crawley, R.A., & Stevenson, R.J. (1990). Reference in single sentences and in texts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 19(3), 191210. doi: 10.1007/BF01077416CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutsch, W., Koster, C., & Koster, J. (1986). What can we learn from children's errors in understanding anaphora? Linguistics, 24(1), 203–26. doi: 10.1515/ling.1986.24.1.203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H. (2004). The acquisition of complex sentences. UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(1/2), 131–51. doi: 10.1515/cogl.2001.006Google Scholar
van Dijk, T.A. (1977). Pragmatic macro-structures in discourse and cognition. In de Mey, M. (Ed.), International workshop on the cognitive viewpoint: CC77 (pp. 99113). University of Ghent.Google Scholar
Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, D.M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Ellis, N.C., & Sinclair, S.G. (1996). Working memory in the acquisition of vocabulary and syntax: putting language in good order. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(1), 234–50. doi: 10.1080/713755604CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiestas, C.E., & Pena, E.D. (2004). Narrative discourse in bilingual children: language and task effects. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 155–68. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2004/016)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, B. (1999). What are discourse markers? Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 931–52. doi: 10.1016/S0378-2166(98)00101-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frizelle, P., Thompson, P.A., McDonald, D., & Bishop, D.V.M. (2018). Growth in syntactic complexity between four years and adulthood: evidence from a narrative task. Journal of Child Language, First View, 124. doi: 10.1017/S0305000918000144Google ScholarPubMed
Gee, J., & Grosjean, F. (1984). Empirical evidence for narrative structure. Cognitive Science, 8, 5985. doi: 10.1016/S0364-0213(84)80025-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillam, R.B, & Pearson, N.A. (2004). Test of narrative language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed, Inc.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. (2001). Theories, language, culture: whorf without wincing. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S.C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 4569). Cambridge, UK: CUP.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S.T. (2009). What is corpus linguistics? Language and Linguistics Compass, 3, 1225–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2009.00149.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M., & Hendriks, H. (1999). Cohesion and anaphora in children's narratives: a comparison of English, French, German, and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 26, 419–52. doi: 10.1017/S0305000999003785CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hickmann, M., Hendriks, H., Roland, F., & Liang, J. (1996). The marking of new information in children's narratives: a comparison of English, French, German, and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 23(3), 591619. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900008965CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J.A., & Shapiro, L.R. (1991). From knowing to telling: the development of children's scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 89136). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1(1), 6185. doi: 10.1080/01690968508402071CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T.A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85(5), 363–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.85.5.363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klee, T., & Fitzgerald, M. D. (1985). The relation between grammatical development and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Child Language, 12(2), 251–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W.J.M. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14, 41104. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90026-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk, third edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Bates, E. (1978). Sentential devices for conveying givenness and newness: a cross-cultural developmental study. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 539–58. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90326-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, M. (1967). A boy, a dog, and a frog. New York: Dial PressGoogle Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1973). Frog on his own. New York: Dial PressGoogle Scholar
Mayer, M., & Mayer, M. (1975). One frog too many. New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (Eds.). (1991). Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
McEnery, T., & Gabrielatos, C. (2006). English corpus linguistics. In Aarts, B. & McMahon, A. (eds,), The handbook of English linguistics (pp. 3371). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGann, W., & Schwartz, A. (1988). Main character in children's narratives. Linguistics, 26, 215–33. doi: 10.1515/ling.1988.26.2.215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merritt, D.D., & Liles, B.Z. (1987). Story grammar ability in children with and without language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 30, 539–52. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3004.539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, M.D., & Brorson, K. (2005). A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLUm) and mean length of utterance in words (MLUw). First Language, 25(3), 365–76. doi: 10.1177/0142723705059114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, R., Hernandez, R., Taylor, L., & Johnson, K. (1997). Narrative development in late talkers: early school age. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(6), 12951303. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1987). The connective ‘and’: do older children use it less as they learn other connectives?. Journal of Child Language, 14(2), 375–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1988). The connective and as discourse glue. First Language, 8(22), 1928. doi: 10.1177/014272378800802202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna, AT: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Redford, M.A. (2013). A comparative analysis of pausing in child and adult storytelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(3), 569–89. doi: 10.1017/s0142716411000877CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanders, T.J.M., Spooren, W.P.M., & Noordman, L.G.M. (1992). Toward a taxonomy of coherence relations. Discourse Processes, 15, 135. doi: 10.1080/01638539209544800CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanders, T.J.M., Spooren, W.P.M., & Noordman, L.G.M. (1993). Coherence relations in a cognitive theory of discourse representation. Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 93134. doi: 10.1515/cogl.1993.4.2.93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiffrin, D. (1987). Discourse markers. UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Serratrice, L. (2007). Referential cohesion in the narratives of bilingual English-Italian children and monolingual peers. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 10581087. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2006.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, L.R., & Hudson, J.A. (1991). Tell me a make-believe story: coherence and cohesion in young children's picture-elicited narratives. Developmental Psychology, 27(6), 960–74. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.6.960CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, H. J., & Fisher, C. (2007). Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children's interpretation of pronouns. Lingua, 117(11), 1959–87. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2006.11.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spooren, W., & Sanders, T. (2008). The acquisition order of coherence relations: on cognitive complexity in discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 40, 2003–26. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2008.04.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tognini-Bonelli, E. (2001). Corpus Linguistics at Work. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warden, D.A. (1976). The influence of context on children's use of identifying expressions and references. British Journal of Psychology, 67(1), 101–12. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1976.tb01501.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warden, D.A. (1981). Learning to identify referents. British Journal of Psychology, 72(1), 93–9. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1981.tb02166.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A.L., & Johnson, C.J. (1993). Relationships between narrative and syntactic competencies in school-aged, hearing-impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14(1), 3559. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400010134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wigglesworth, G. (1990). Children's narrative acquisition: a study of some aspects of reference and anaphora. First Language, 29, 105–25. doi: 10.1177/014272379001002902CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwicky, A.M. (1985). Clitics and particles. Language, 61(2), 283-305. doi: 10.2307/414146CrossRefGoogle Scholar