Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T22:12:43.310Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children’s engagement and caregivers’ use of language-boosting strategies during shared book reading: A mixed methods approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 August 2022

Jamie LINGWOOD*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Liverpool Hope University, UK
Sofia LAMPROPOULOU
Affiliation:
Department of English, University of Liverpool, UK
Christophe DE BEZENAC
Affiliation:
Institute of Systems, Molecular and Integrative Biology (ISMIB), University of Liverpool
Josie BILLINGTON
Affiliation:
Department of English, University of Liverpool, UK
Caroline ROWLAND
Affiliation:
Language Development Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, UK Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition & Behaviour, Radboud University, Netherlands
*
Corresponding author. Dr. Jamie Lingwood, Department of Psychology, Liverpool Hope University, L16 9JD, UK E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

For shared book reading to be effective for language development, the adult and child need to be highly engaged. The current paper adopted a mixed-methods approach to investigate caregiver’s language-boosting behaviours and children’s engagement during shared book reading. The results revealed there were more instances of joint attention and caregiver’s use of prompts during moments of higher engagement. However, instances of most language-boosting behaviours were similar across episodes of higher and lower engagement. Qualitative analysis assessing the link between children’s engagement and caregiver’s use of speech acts, revealed that speech acts do seem to contribute to high engagement, in combination with other aspects of the interaction.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The goal of the present paper was to use a mixed-methods approach to investigate caregiver’s language-boosting behaviours and children’s engagement during shared book reading, and to determine the role these factors might play in explaining why shared book reading is such an effective child language promoting tool.

Shared book reading is the act of an adult sharing a book with a child or group of children (Noble et al., Reference Noble, Sala, Lowe, Lingwood, Rowland, Gobet and Pine2019). There are many different forms of shared book reading, one of which is interactive shared book reading, which involves the adult using a specific set of techniques to create a conversation about the book, as opposed to simply reading the text aloud. Interactive shared book reading has been shown to support a range of early language skills including vocabulary (e.g., Elley, Reference Elley1989; Farrant & Zubrick, Reference Farrant and Zubrick2013), narrative and conversation skills (e.g., Morrow, Reference Morrow1988; Reese, Reference Reese1995), future reading ability (e.g., Bus, van IJzendoorn & Pellegrini, Reference Bus, Van Ijzendoorn and Pellegrini1995), print awareness (e.g., Justice & Ezell, Reference Justice and Ezell2000, Reference Justice and Ezell2004), grammatical development (e.g., Valdez-Menchaca & Whitehurst, Reference Valdez-Menchaca and Whitehurst1992; Whitehurst, Falco, Lonigan, Fischel, DeBaryshe, Valdez-Menchaca & Caulfield, Reference Whitehurst, Falco, Lonigan, Fischel, DeBaryshe, Valdez-Menchaca and Caulfield1988) and phonological awareness (e.g., Chow, McBride-Chang, Cheung & Chow, Reference Chow, McBride-Chang, Cheung and Chow2008; Lefebvre, Trudeau & Sutton, Reference Lefebvre, Trudeau and Sutton2011). Shared book reading exposes children to a wide variety of complex sentence structures, and lexically diverse language (Dawson, Hasio, Tan, Banerji & Nation, Reference Dawson, Hasio, Tan, Banerji and Nation2021; Montag, Jones & Smith, Reference Montag, Jones and Smith2015). Furthermore, compared to other contexts such as free play, during shared book reading, caregivers have been found to produce higher levels of lexically and syntactically diverse child-directed speech (Noble, Cameron-Faulkner & Lieven, Reference Noble, Cameron-Faulkner and Lieven2018).

Another reason that interactive shared book reading supports children’s language development is that it provides a good context for learning language. During interactive shared book reading, the child and adult are said to be in joint attention with one another: that is, they are engaging in ‘the visual sharing of attention with a social partner, in reference to an object or event of mutual interest’ (Carpenter, Nagell, Tomasello, Butterworth & Moore, Reference Carpenter, Nagell, Tomasello, Butterworth and Moore1998). This has been linked to positive language outcomes in preschoolers (Farrant & Zubrick, Reference Farrant and Zubrick2013; Rees, Nadig & Rvachew, Reference Rees, Nadig and Rvachew2017). Having a clear joint attentional focus during shared book reading also provides the opportunity for the child and adult to engage in conversation. For example, it encourages the child to take turns and to produce and practice language, as well as providing an opportunity for the adult to model language boosting behaviours such as prompts, open questions, descriptions, expansions and recasts. The evidence also shows that parents tend to talk more, ask more questions, and use more non-immediate or decontextualised talk during shared book reading than in other play contexts (e.g., Curenton, Craig & Flanigan, Reference Curenton, Craig and Flanigan2008; Korat, Reference Korat2009; Leech, Salo, Rowe & Cabrera, Reference Leech, Salo, Rowe and Cabrera2013).

Strategies like those described above also encourage children’s active involvement. For example, techniques such as prompting the child with questions, expanding the child’s responses, and asking open questions are all designed to engage children (Bojczyk, Davis & Rana, Reference Bojczyk, Davis and Rana2016). The quality of the interaction and how engaged the child is during shared book reading is, therefore, somewhat determined by what the adult does. Describing what is happening in the book, initiating turntaking, using wh-questions, and attentional directives during shared book reading have all been shown to promote children’s language development (Bojczyk et al., Reference Bojczyk, Davis and Rana2016; Fletcher, Cross, Tanney, Schneider & Finch, Reference Fletcher, Cross, Tanney, Schneider and Finch2008; Pine, Reference Pine1991; Romeo et al., Reference Romeo, Leonard, Robinson, West, Mackey and Rowe2018; Snow et al., Reference Snow, Arlman-Rupp, Hassing, Jobse, Joosten and Vorster1976). Conversely, asking fewer open-ended questions (Crowe, Reference Crowe2000), using more direct attentives (Bojczyk et al., Reference Bojczyk, Davis and Rana2016) and behavioural regulation strategies (Chang & Luo, Reference Chang and Luo2020) can limit conversational opportunities (though for an alternative view, see Pine, Reference Pine1991; Son & Tineo, Reference Son and Tineo2016).

What is clear from the literature is that the highest quality shared book reading interactions rely upon reciprocal interactions to scaffold children’s learning (Fleury & Hugh, Reference Fleury and Hugh2018). High quality shared book reading interactions encourage children to ask questions, make predictions, form inferences, which then leads children to use and hear different new words (Chang & Luo, Reference Chang and Luo2020; Hindman, Connor, Jewkes & Morrison, Reference Hindman, Connor, Jewkes and Morrison2008). To describe these processes, we can borrow terms from the sociolinguistics literature: shared reading can be viewed as a ‘transactional framework’ in which the caregiver and child are operating as a community of practice (CofP) during shared book reading.

A CofP is defined within the field of sociolinguistics as “an aggregate of people who come together around mutual engagements in an endeavour. Ways of doing things, ways of talking, beliefs, values, power relations – in short, practices – emerge during this mutual endeavour. As a social construct, a CofP is different from the traditional community, primarily because it is defined by its membership and by the practice in which that membership engages” (Eckert & McConnell-Ginet, Reference Eckert and McConnell-Ginet1992, p. 464). This definition implies that practices, be they linguistic or social, are negotiated and shared through interaction and engagement in a joint goal which, in our case, is shared reading. The definition and configuration of a CofP derives from the activities in which the members are involved, which thus certify their common bonds. A CofP involves the following three dimensions (Wenger, Reference Wenger1998, pp. 76-85):

1 Mutual engagement

This involves dense relations and regular interaction. When it comes to a main carer/child relationship, spending time together and jointly engaging in activities gives rise to shared experiences and an (inevitable) mutual engagement in different types of endeavours (e.g., shopping, bath time, bedtime routine, playing, cooking to name a few).

2 A joint enterprise

This relates to a shared goal, but it is not just a stated shared goal. It is the result of a collective process of negotiation. Shared reading works in this way as carers voluntarily agree to the intervention and carer/child mutually become involved in the shared reading activity. As we discuss in the qualitative analysis below, every dyad engaged with the activity differently and observed moments of low to high engagement are a result of this process of joint negotiation.

3 A shared repertoire

This is the discourse associated with being a member of the community and is acquired throughout the process of being a member and negotiating one’s membership. It includes ways of saying, doing, thinking and acting in the world as well as shared values which are produced and reproduced through language. The concept of CofP shows how members of a group/community share ways of thinking, doing and acting, since a CofP is seen as “a group of people whose joint engagement in an enterprise is sufficiently intensive to give rise over time to a repertoire of shared practices” (Eckert and McConnell-Ginet, Reference Eckert and McConnell-Ginet1999, p. 185). A shared repertoire develops during shared reading between a caregiver and their child and reflects a similar worldview. Aspects of such a repertoire could include humorous strategies such as laughing at specific activities, repetition and uptake of each other’s words and phrases but also wider evaluative comments and paralinguistic features.

Additionally, it is important to consider the role of the book itself. Although an under researched aspect of shared book reading, there is evidence that different elements of a book can provide children with different learning opportunities. For example, the use of an informational text in shared book reading can encourage more cognitively challenging discussions between children and adults (Price, Bradley & Smith, Reference Price, Bradley and Smith2012), whereas books with illustrations have been shown to aid children’s comprehension of those illustrations (Mantei & Kervin, Reference Mantei and Kervin2014). In the current study an array of different age-appropriate books were used but with varied styles and word counts.

To summarise, interactive shared reading boosts children’s language development partly because, during shared book reading, the adult and child work together in a CofP with a clear joint attentional focus. This provides the opportunity for the child and adult to engage in real conversation and as a result, the adult can model language boosting behaviours such as prompts, open questions, descriptives, expansions and recasts, whilst producing more varied vocabulary and complex sentence structures.

However, what is often not considered in the shared reading literature is the fact that, for all the above to apply, the child and adult both need to be highly engaged in the book reading task. If either the child or the adult is not engaged (e.g., engagement is low, they are distracted, or not really listening), then it is unlikely that the adult gets the opportunity to model these language boosting behaviours, or that the child gets the opportunity to benefit from them. Given that shared book reading interactions are often of a reciprocal nature, it is thus important to understand both the caregiver’s and the child’s role in the interaction.

The first aim of this paper was, thus, to investigate what characterises high and low engagement during shared book reading and how engagement results from the quality of the interaction. In the past, engagement has been measured by concepts like child interest (Cline, Reference Cline2010; Moody, Justice & Cabell, Reference Moody, Justice and Cabell2010) and components have included sustained visual attention to the storybook, coordinated attention to the parent, and verbal communication related to book content (Richter & Courage, Reference Richter and Courage2017). Those studies that have measured engagement have often employed static summative measures across the entire interaction, but the issue with such static measures is that they do not necessarily capture the dynamic aspects of engagement – namely, how engagement fluctuates over the duration of the interaction (Wicks, Paynter & Westerveld, Reference Wicks, Paynter and Westerveld2020).

Additionally, very few studies have investigated the link between children’s engagement and parental behaviours during shared book reading. One of the few studies to do so reported that, in a sample of 51 low-income families, mother’s use of attention-getting utterances was associated with children’s verbal engagement (as measured by several indicators; Son & Tineo, Reference Son and Tineo2016). Conversely, visual engagement (measured by parent’s and child’s combined looks to the storybook) was not associated with mother’s use of attention-getting utterances. However, Son and Tineo employed static engagement measures and did not explore the link between engagement and any other language-boosting behaviours during shared book reading, other than attention-getting utterances.

Only one study to our knowledge explored the relationship between engagement and several parents’ language-boosting behaviours (Wicks et al., Reference Wicks, Paynter and Westerveld2020). They measured parents’ (i) use of book vocabulary, (ii) teaching of print-related skills, (iii) explicit teaching of story structure, and (iv) use of questions, in a sample of 40 preschoolers on the autistic spectrum. They found strong significant associations between children’s visual attention, verbal engagement, and parents’ use of questions and prompts during shared book reading. But again, verbal engagement was a static summative measure that may not adequately capture how engagement and parental language-boosting behaviours are related to one another, throughout the interaction.

Therefore, to date there has been very little work that acknowledges the role of dynamic measures of engagement and language-boosting behaviours in book reading interactions. As a result, it is not always clear how much children are engaging during shared book reading and what effect this may have on the quality of the interaction. In the current study, we measured how engaged children were during thirty second intervals, so that we got a measure of how engagement changed dynamically across the interaction.

The second aim of this paper was to identify the strategies that adults and children are using to encourage moments of high engagement. This will be explored through a CofP framework to establish the interactive and conversational features that are used by dyads, and how they are used to set up a successful CofP. In the context of shared book reading, it has been argued that engagement is a dynamic process that fluctuates throughout the interaction (Fleury & Hugh, Reference Fleury and Hugh2018; Ortiz, Stowe & Arnold, Reference Ortiz, Stowe and Arnold2001). Therefore, there is an argument that engagement may not necessarily be tied to one variable. Related to this, Kucirkova, Messer and Whitelock (Reference Kucirkova, Messer and Whitelock2013) have previously advocated that engagement should be measured with a range of qualitative and quantitative techniques. Qualitative approaches to shared book reading have focused on shared reading practices (Levy, Hall & Preece, Reference Levy, Hall and Preece2018), and barriers to reading (Lingwood, Levy, Billington & Rowland, Reference Lingwood, Levy, Billington and Rowland2020b; Preece & Levy, Reference Preece and Levy2018), but no studies to our knowledge have investigated children’s engagement during shared book reading using a qualitative approach. To address this, we will use a qualitative approach to assess the link between children’s engagement and caregiver’s language practices during shared book reading.

In the current study, six dyads took part in a pre-intervention shared reading session where they were asked, using books provided by the researcher, to read with their child as they would normally for 10 minutes. Following this, they then took part in a Shared Reading ‘intervention’ group run by a local charity called the The Reader once a week for eight weeks. These families then took part in a post-intervention shared reading session where they were again asked to read with their child for 10 minutes the same books provided at pre-intervention stage. The shared reading sessions were video-recorded for offline coding and analysis.

Our research questions were as followsFootnote 1:

Research Question 1: Are there more language-boosting behaviours in moments of high engagements than low engagement? The language-boosting behaviours were all based on previous coding schemes in the wider language development literature. We predicted that there would be more language-boosting opportunities in periods of high engagement such as longer joint attention, increased number of caregiver turns, desirable speech acts, and desirable speech act responses (see Tables 2 & 4 for more details on how language-boosting behaviours were chosen and categorised).

Research Question 2: Using a qualitative analysis approach, can we identify strategies that adults and children, together, use in interaction to produce moments of high engagement; i.e., can we establish what interactive and conversational features are used by dyads to set up a successful CofP.

Method

Sample

For this study, six primary caregivers and their 3- to 4-year old children (M = 44.83 months, SD = 4.22 months) were chosen for in-depth analysis from a wider sample of 85 families who took part in a randomised control trial testing the effectiveness of The Reader’s shared reading programme (see Lingwood, Billington & Rowland, Reference Lingwood, Billington and Rowland2020a). Demographic details are displayed in Table 1. Participants were recruited from preschools in socially deprived areas of a large Northern city in the UK, as measured by English Indices of Deprivation (IMD), preschool size and Ofsted inspection and regulation ratings. Ethical approval was granted by the University of (Anonymised) ethics committee. All participating caregivers gave informed consent. At the end of the study, all caregivers were reimbursed with vouchers and books.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of sample at baseline. Numbers refer to mean (SD) for Children’s age, and N (%) for all other rows.

Materials

We identified four popular children’s books for child and caregiver to read together during the shared book reading session. We wanted to choose books which families would not necessarily be familiar with. Therefore, we excluded any book titles or authors who had appeared in the title and author checklists that caregivers filled out, as described in Lingwood et al. (Reference Lingwood, Billington and Rowland2020a). Taking these restrictions into account, the following children’s books were used during the shared book reading session: King Jack and the Dragon by Peter Bently (Bently, Reference Bently2011); Beautiful Bananas by Elizabeth Laird (Laird, Reference Laird2004); The Storm Whale by Benji Davies (Davies, Reference Davies2011); and Goodnight Gorilla by Peggy Rathmann (Rathmann, Reference Rathmann1994).

A video camera on a gorilla pod was used to capture the shared reading session. The camera was usually placed towards the back of the living room, rather than directly in front of the caregiver and child.

Procedure

Primary caregivers and their children were a subsample of those who took part in a larger study that is described in Lingwood et al. (Reference Lingwood, Billington and Rowland2020a). The aim of that study was to evaluate how families from lower SES backgrounds responded to The Reader’s shared reading intervention: which emphasised the enjoyment of reading, rather than its educational value. In Lingwood et al., 85 lower SES families and their 3- to 4-year old children from 10 different preschools were randomly allocated to take part in The Reader’s shared reading programme (intervention) or an existing ‘Story Time’ group at a library (control), once a week for eight weeks. Those who participated in the intervention group (n = 43) were additionally invited to take part in the current study, in which they were asked to read with their 3- to 4-year old child in the home as they would normally, for 10 minutes, before and after the intervention. Six families consented to take part in the study. A trained research assistant visited each family in the home to conduct the shared reading session, before (pre-) and after (post-) the intervention.

At the start of the first home visit, the research assistant spent 5-10 minutes establishing rapport with the caregiver and child, before going through the information sheet with caregivers, to ensure they were aware of what the study involved. Once caregivers were happy to participate, they signed a consent form. The research assistant asked the caregiver to read the books provided with their child, as they would normally, for 10 minutesFootnote 3. They were also told that they could read the books in any order they wished, and did not need to read all of them. The research assistant positioned the camera on a tripod towards the back of the room, and started recording before leaving the room.

Measures

Language boosting behaviour coding

During shared book reading, we coded incidents of turn taking, head direction, caregiver’s speech acts, and caregiver’s speech acts responses. These behaviours were all based on previous coding schemes in the wider language development literature. Turntaking was based on Snow and Ferguson (Reference Snow and Ferguson1977); head direction was based on Cameron-Faulkner (Reference Cameron-Faulkner2014); speech acts were based on the categories derived from Pine (Reference Pine1991); and speech act responses were based on Chouinard and Clark (Reference Chouinard and Clark2003). A full description of these tiers is shown in Table 2. Coding was done in ELAN (ELAN, version 5.2). In order to measure the inter-rater reliability of the behavioural coding scheme, a second researcher, unaware of condition, coded two of the videos (one pre-intervention, one post-intervention). The coding was almost identical, with 99% agreement between videos.

Table 2. Coding scheme used to measure turntaking, head direction, speech acts, and speech act responses during shared book readingFootnote 4

Engagement coding

We used the Leuven Scale of Active Engagement (Laevers, Reference Laevers2008) to measure how engaged children were during the shared book reading sessions, at thirty second intervals. This was chosen as an appropriate interval so that we could measure how engagement changes across the interaction, rather than just at one time point. The Leuven Scale of Active Engagement assesses how engaged children are when completing a particular activity such as book reading. As shown in Table 3, the scale ranges from 1 (extremely low engagement) to 5 (extremely high engagement) and the scale contains a number of examples demonstrating the varying levels of engagement. A research assistant conducted the engagement coding for each of the videos and did not know which videos were pre- and post-intervention. To check the inter-rater reliability of the engagement coding, a second researcher checked two of the videos (one pre-intervention, one post-intervention). The coding was almost identical and where there were disagreements in scores, these were resolved.

Table 3. The Leuven Scale of Active Engagement, taken from Laevers (Reference Laevers2008)

Analysis approach

The first author used a quantitative approach to analyse (i) children’s engagement scores at each 30 second interval, (ii) the proportion of time spent in each language boosting behaviour for periods of high and low engagement. Data and code can be found online: https://osf.io/2pdm3/

The same twelve shared book reading videos were analysed by the second author using a qualitative approach. We focused on the transitions from low to high engagement to examine whether these transitions were associated with specific parental behaviour. We transcribed these extracts by adapting the Jefferson transcription system. Transcription conventions are included in the results section. We conducted a discourse analysis of these transition moments and analysed them in their local context of occurrence (i.e., the whole video). Specifically, we focused on linguistic features such as collaborative overlaps and rising intonation that create a high involvement style (Tannen, Reference Tannen1994) and analytical tools from narrative discourse (Georgakopoulou, Reference Georgakopoulou1997; Lampropoulou, Reference Lampropoulou2012; Ochs & Capps, Reference Ochs and Capps2001) to interpret collaborative moments during the act of storytelling. To do this we looked at several moments of very low engagement (coded in the Leuven coding scheme as scores of 1) and we observed the caregiver’s last utterance in the 30 second clip, and noted what this speech act was (e.g., wh-question, behavioural directive, attentional directive, closed question etc). We then looked at what the next engagement score was, and whether it changed from low engagement (coded in the Leuven coding scheme as scores of 1 or 2) to high engagement (coded in the Leuven coding scheme as scores of 4 or 5).

Results

Research question 1

Our first aim was to examine whether caregivers produced more language-boosting behaviours in episodes of high engagement than in episodes of low engagement. The language-boosting behaviours we examined were joint attention, caregiver turns, descriptions, WH-questions, prompts, expansions, and attentional directives (see Table 4 for full details). We note that there is mixed evidence for whether attentional directives are ‘language-boosting’.

Table 4. Identifying which behaviours are associated with low and high engagement from the wider literature. Attentional directives have been characterised as both low and high engagement in the literature

Note: See Table 2 for a full description of tiers and behaviours.

Leuven engagement scores ranged between 1 to 5 and were collected at every 30 second interval during the interaction. We then calculated the proportion of time during each 30 second interval that was spent in each language boosting behaviour. This gave us, for each behaviour, an average proportion calculated over the number of 30 second intervals. We then used the Leuven engagement category descriptions, which states that scores of 4 and 5 are classified as ‘high/very high’, to binarise scores into low engagement [scores of 1, 2, 3] and high engagement [scores of 4, 5]Footnote 5. We then computed the proportion of time spent in each language boosting behaviour for periods of high and low engagement separately for each participant, before averaging across pre and post intervention sessions within participants. Finally, we ran a paired permutation t-test to compare the proportion of time spent in each language boosting behaviour when engagement ratings were high and low. The permutation approach is a common approach in corpus linguistics (Pastor & Seghiri, Reference Pastor and Seghiri2010) that allows us to analyse data from small samples where there are a lot of datapoints per participant. In the current study we have numerous datapoints per participant which enables us to run a parametric test on a small sample size (n = 6) because it avoids making assumptions about parametric distributions which are difficult to make with smaller sample sizes. This approach has been shown to be optimal in comparison with samples with larger numbers of participants (Bowker & Pearson, Reference Bowker and Pearson2002; Koch, Reference Koch1997, Reference Koch2001), giving us the statistical power that we need, but bypassing the assumptions that usually apply.

On average, children were in levels of ‘high’ engagement 58% of the time (SD = 50%) [Participant 1: M = 55%, SD = 41%; Participant 2: M = 75%, SD = 40%; Participant 3: M = 30%, SD = 46%; Participant 4: M = 58%, SD = 37%; Participant 5: M = 50%, SD = 43%; Participant 6: M = 83%, SD = 24%]. The results of this analysis are illustrated in Figure 1. We found low and high engagement differences in the proportion of joint attention (JA) towards the book (t(5) = −7.44, p < .05, mean difference = 0.31), suggesting that children and adults were more likely to focus their joint attention towards the book in periods of high engagement. However, note that this is, in fact, a circular finding, since high engagement was defined, in part, as both child and adult paying attention to an activity which, in this case, was defined by the experimenter as book reading. More interestingly, we also found that adults were more likely to use prompts in periods of high engagement (t(5) = −2.61, p < .05, mean difference = 0.02), than low engagement, though the mean difference effect size was very small. We found no other differences in proportion of time spent eliciting language boosting behaviours for low and high engagement periods (all p-values >.05, see Table 5).

Figure 1. The paired mean difference between low and high engagement is shown in a series of Gardner-Altman estimation plots below. Both groups are plotted on the X axes as a slopegraph: each paired set of observations is connected by a line. The paired mean difference is plotted on a floating axis on the right as a bootstrap sampling distribution. Mean difference is depicted as a dot. The 95% confidence interval is indicated by the ends of the vertical error bar.

Table 5. T-scores, P-values, and mean difference effect size for proportion of time spent in each language-boosting behaviour across moments of low and high engagement.

Research question 2

Our second research question asked if we could identify strategies that adult and children use to produce moments of high engagement. The twelve shared book reading videos were analysed by the second author using a discourse analysis of selected extracts that included transitions from low to high engagement. The findings were interpreted using a CofP approach that views the shared reading activity as an interactional process between caregiver and child throughout which a repertoire of shared practices develops. This repertoire is different for every dyad observed and reflects different social practices as well as different negotiating techniques that lead to the accomplishment of the activity.

For example, in video 3: [participant 117, post intervention)] the caregiver developed a narrating pattern that was mainly characterised by shifts in pitch – namely, from rising to low intonation, loud voice as well as WH- questions that enabled the child to comment on the storytelling world. We specifically analysed the strategies used by the caregiver in moments of movement from low to high engagement. In particular, from 12.10 minutes to 13.10 minutes we observed a transition from low (coded as 2 in the quantitative analysis) to high engagement (coded as 4 and then 5 in the quantitative analysis).

Transcription conventions are as follows:

While the caregiver in turn 1 is dramatizing the story through loud speech and rising intonation, the child interrupts her by asking her to remove the ribbon out of her hair. This can be seen as a moment of low engagement as both child and caregiver temporarily stop being involved with the reading activity. However, the caregiver quickly responds to the child’s request and immediately shifts back her attention to reading in turn 3 by dramatizing the narrative, mainly through rising intonation, which is immediately followed by a direct, WH-question “what do you think they’re gonna make”: In line with our quantitative analysis findings, dyads were in periods of low and high engagement with equal frequency (engagement was high 58% of the time, on average). The child’s immediate involvement is evidenced in turn 4 “a big castle” which consists of the response to the caregiver’s question. The response here is given promptly, without delay. This indicates the child is following the story and the manner of her answer – namely, loud speech, signals excitement. In line with the quantitative findings, dyads are in joint attention, and high engagement is mostly maintained, throughout the video, via direct, WH- questions on the part of the caregiver to which the child responds.

Another involving strategy can be seen in turn 5 where the caregiver encourages the child to see the picture book “oh look at the castle”. The child uptakes this attentional directive invitation; instead of merely repeating the caregiver’s sentence, she expands upon it by adding an evaluative statement to the plot in turn 6 “the baby will be happy”. This consists of an assessment of a book character. She has retrieved this information from the story itself with which she seems to be familiar. The caregiver moves along the same topic (R-MOVE) and follows up the child’s evaluative statement with further character assessment in turn 7 “I know they look all excited don’t they” to which the child agrees in turn 8. It follows that caregiver and child do not only read from the book but they also talk about the story itself by hypothetically portraying the represented characters’ emotions.

The above shared reading practices reflect a high involvement style (Tannen, Reference Tannen1994) giving priority to the need of ‘positive face’ (Brown & Levinson, Reference Brown and Levinson1987), to show involvement and familiarity. We argue that this high involvement style is a resource that reflects, among other things, familiarity with and pleasure in reading, which characterises the specific dyad. Overall, the videos of the specific dyad display mostly high engagement which is evidenced via several strategies that were only qualitatively considered as adding to the contextual understanding of the dyad’s shared practices. Specifically, the caregiver engages the child via shifting pitch, dramatizing narrative and quotations, and formulating direct questions which the child consistently reciprocated. Other parameters of high engagement include the child being familiar with the stories and having developed her own preferences, as evidenced by, among other behaviours, the immediacy with which she responds to the reading options her carer offers earlier in the video. Additionally, the child often offers evaluative statements in response to the caregiver as in turn 6; this shows they have practised shared reading before and they both know what to expect from the activity.

High engagement is achieved via different strategies in the rest of the dataset and among different CofPs. For example, in video 6 [participant 40, post-intervention] both intensity and pace of intonation characterise the caregiver’s reading style which is, primarily, performance oriented. Specifically, she chooses to vividly imitate different animal voices while reading. In accordance with the quantitative analysis, caregivers’ speech acts were categorised as reading the text (S-ROUTINES) or not reading the text, which included all other non-reading behaviours e.g., WH-questions, descriptions, attentional directives etc. But qualitatively, a specific pattern can be observed in her reading style; she clearly distinguishes, mainly via changes in pitch and intonation, between reading out the story plot, quoting speech and addressing the child. She also uses a lot of paralinguistic features to imitate the action described in the story. The next extract is representative of how the caregiver handles a specific instance of low engagement in moment 6’30 to 7’30.

The above transcript represents a transition from moments of high engagement to low and back to high. The exchange begins with the adult prompting the child to say something. As described in the quantitative findings, prompts occurred more often in periods of higher engagement. But it may also be that the overlap itself between caregiver and child in turns 2 and 3 also indicates high engagement. Specifically, the child not only finishes off the caregiver’s prompt, but the overlap is done in a loud voice signalling excitement about the complicating action part of the story (Labov, Reference Labov1972). This can be seen as a collaborative interruption (Tannen, Reference Tannen1983; Murata, Reference Murata1994) that contributes to the advancement of the plot or the addition of elements of orientation and/or of evaluative comments. As Ochs and Capps (Reference Ochs and Capps2001, p. 2) point out ‘the difference between telling a story to another and telling a story with another is an important one’. In the latter case, ‘narrative becomes an interactional achievement and interlocutors become co-authors’.

The caregiver continues narrating via systematically changing pitch (rising intonation) every time she quotes the story characters’ voices which is followed by falling intonation every time she describes the characters’ physical actions. Additionally, she consistently points to the book when physical action is described and uses paralinguistic features (hand gestures) to selectively imitate physical action i.e., swooping. Quantitatively, this might suggest that they are in joint attention with one another. However, the child does not seem to be fully engaged at this stage as he does not contribute to the reading activity. So, the caregiver attempts to attract his attention by commenting on the plot in turn 3 “he doesn’t know does he ((points book))”. This can be seen as an indirect prompt as the child is not explicitly invited to respond to it. Indeed, the child does not follow-up this indirect prompt. Then, the caregiver chooses a relevant transition point to use a wh- question “what colour are his feathers on his tail ((points book)) can you see” to which the child immediately responds in turn 4. The caregiver evaluates the response in a positive way in turn 5. Subsequently, high engagement is maintained in turns 6 and 7 when the child uninvited provides an assessment of the described story plot and the caregiver responds to it. At this moment, the dyad is talking about the plot in a contextualised way rather than merely reading out.

The above analysis showed that engagement is a dynamic process that is constructed moment to moment in the local reading context. It is also a result of negotiation as evidenced by the caregivers’ first failed attempt to actively engage the child: which had to be revised later on. The reading practices the specific dyad has developed not only reflect their exposure to reading in the past but also their mutual engagement in several other activities that has given rise to shared understandings of said activity’s norms and expectations.

Discussion

Our first research question explored whether language-boosting behaviours occurred more frequently in moments of higher engagement during shared book reading. We found evidence to suggest that during these periods, (i) caregiver and child’s head direction tended to be towards the book and (ii) caregivers’ increased their use of prompts. However, no other language-boosting behaviours occurred more often in periods of higher engagement.

All other language-boosting behaviours remained stable between moments of high and low engagement. This is in contrast with the small degree of evidence from previous literature – namely, Son and Tineo (Reference Son and Tineo2016), who found that mother’s use of attention-getting utterances was associated with children’s overall verbal engagement and Wicks et al. (Reference Wicks, Paynter and Westerveld2020) who found strong significant associations between children’s visual attention, verbal engagement, and parents’ use of questions and prompts during shared book reading. There is no consensus on how engagement is operationalised and so this may explain the difference in findings between these studies and the current study. In particular, in the current study we used a broad engagement measure, whereas both Son & Tineo and Wicks et al. both measured whether children were not engaged or fully engaged on several indicators such as text reading, story description, story inference, and responsivity.

Notwithstanding this, our quantitative results suggest that the dynamic between child and adult is more subtle and active than we predicted (Fleury & Hugh, Reference Fleury and Hugh2018; Ortiz et al., Reference Ortiz, Stowe and Arnold2001). In other words, it is not the case that being in a higher state of engagement allows parents to use language boosting behaviours or conversely that such language boosting behaviours inevitably encourage higher engagement. Rather, the process of shared reading is more nuanced whereby engagement fluctuates over the course of the interaction. This suggests that the caregiver may dynamically use several strategies (i) in response to child’s lack of attention/focus, to encourage higher engagement, as well as (ii) during periods of high engagement. Importantly, many of these strategies are also what we would categorise as language boosting behaviours (e.g., asking questions), which may explain why we found no difference between language boosting behaviours in high and low engagement scenarios, because caregivers are using these as attention getters in moments of low engagement, as well as these being ways of teaching language during moments of high engagement. This suggests the role of such strategies may be multipurpose – that they don’t just fulfil the role of modelling language but of promoting engagement too.

Our qualitative analysis confirmed that caregivers were using several other strategies to engage their child that were not captured in our quantitative analysis. Drawing upon the transactional framework literature, we assessed whether changes in engagement from low to high coincide with associated changes in caregivers’ speech acts, i.e., whether certain desirable speech acts are effective in turning moments of low engagement into moments of high engagement. Our qualitative analysis enabled us to understand in more detail the relationship between engagement and speech acts.

Collectively, the quantitative and qualitative findings showed that every caregiver engages with different strategies (as described in the sample analysis of two selected videos above) that appeal to their child, and that every caregiver is responsive to the child’s preferences, abilities, and overall behaviour during reading. At the same time, the child responds to caregiver’s prompts and recognises them as cues for maintaining shared reading attention. The cues not only contextualise the story book itself but also the wider shared reading activity, pointing to how it is meant to be perceived and negotiated in the local context of interaction. Therefore, every dyad seems to be working as a CofP who developed their own understandings and shared repertoire through exposure and mutual engagement in several other activities, the intervention itself being one of them. Negotiating strategies are shared between caregiver and child and reflect common values, practices and understanding of the shared reading activity. High engagement is relative to the CofP’s shared understanding of book reading (reading for pleasure could be one). As a result, the type of speech acts we coded and quantified in our quantitative analysis (e.g., wh-question, behavioural directive, attentional directive, closed question etc) are tools, among many others, and in combination with others we observed qualitatively, that seem to contribute to high engagement. In other words, engagement is a complex process that cannot be predicted by and is not tied to individual variables; rather it is dynamically constructed in the local reading context. We have shown, for example, that a caregiver may often revise an engagement strategy e.g., dramatisation through constructed dialogue followed by performative reading. This is an indicator that caregivers respond to the local context that is dynamically constructed and negotiated throughout the process of shared book reading.

One aspect of the shared reading interaction that we have not considered in detail in this paper is the contribution of the book itself. The books that were shared by dyads in the current study varied on a number of variables such as content, genre, illustrations, and word count. Therefore, in addition to considering inter-caregiver differences in engagement strategies, it is important to also consider how differences in book might prompt certain language-boosting behaviours. For example, in ‘Beautiful Bananas’, the main character meets a number of animals. This may have encouraged the caregiver to use attentional directives, which attract the child’s attention to each of the animals. Conversely, although animals featured heavily in ‘Goodnight Gorilla’, this book contains very few words and so it is likely that caregivers may have used more descriptions to describe what was happening to the child. Finally, ‘The Storm Whale’ is a tender and heart-warming story about friendship and may have encouraged the caregiver to use WH-questions to probe the child on some of the emotions that characters were feeling. Exploring how the various components of a book influence the interaction is beyond the scope of this paper, it should be an avenue for future research.

We also note that there are some other limitations that should be borne in mind when interpreting our results. These shared reading interactions give a snapshot of shared book reading in the home on a day. We therefore recognise that the snapshot we recorded may not have necessarily been representative of each caregiver-child shared book reading interaction. Additionally, it is worth reiterating that the six dyads who participated in the current study were a self-selecting sample from a cohort of forty-three eligible families. Dyads were arguably all confident and well versed in the practice of shared book reading, whereas we know that not all families are (McCarthey, Reference McCarthey1997). Therefore, the findings from these six dyads may not necessarily be representative of other dyads – we have considered barriers to shared book reading in this project elsewhere, in Lingwood et al. (Reference Lingwood, Levy, Billington and Rowland2020b).

Summary

Although there were more instances of joint attention and caregiver’s use of prompts during moments of higher engagement, instances of most language-boosting behaviours were similar across episodes of higher and lower engagement. Our qualitative findings suggest that caregivers are subtly navigating the child’s engagement state; they are using language boosting behaviours throughout the interaction not just in moments of high engagement, responding when the child loses engagement, and taking advantage of high engagement. Caregivers need to use several strategies in response to a child’s lack of attention/focus during low engagement, to encourage a move toward higher engagement, as well as sustaining these strategies during periods of high engagement.

Appendices

Appendix 1. Mean proportion of time in language boosting behaviours across engagement levels. Standard deviations are in parentheses.

Inspection of the means in appendix 1 reveal a similar pattern of findings to the main analysis that was conducted for research question 1. That is to say that the proportion of time spent in language boosting behaviour was similar across level of engagement. The only exception to this was ‘joint attention with the book’ where the higher the engagement score, the more the larger the mean proportion of time spent in join attention with the book.

Footnotes

1 In an original version of the manuscript we also tested 2 other hypotheses. These analyses were removed after the 1st round of reviews, but are available on Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/76x23/

2 GCSEs are exams taken at the end of British high school when the student is 15 or 16 years old. Five passes at Grade C or higher are considered roughly equivalent to a U.S. high school diploma. A-Level exams are generally taken 2 years later when students are 17 or 18 years old. A-Levels are most similar to American Advanced Placement courses.

3 N=1 dyad did not complete the full ten minutes of shared book reading for the pre-intervention visit and therefore their recording was terminated after seven minutes. Their data is included in all analyses.

4 These behaviours were all based on previous coding schemes in the wider language development literature. Turntaking was based on Snow and Ferguson (Reference Snow and Ferguson1977); head direction was based on Cameron-Faulkner (2010); speech acts were based on the categories derived from Pine (Reference Pine1991); and speech act responses were based on Chouinard and Clark (Reference Chouinard and Clark2003).

5 An additional analysis without binarization revealed similar results to our analysis reported for research question 1 (see appendix 1).

References

Bently, P. (2011). King Jack and the Dragon. London: Puffin.Google Scholar
Bojczyk, K. E., Davis, A. E., & Rana, V. (2016). Mother–child interaction quality in shared book reading: Relation to child vocabulary and readiness to read. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 404414. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowker, L., & Pearson, J. (2002). Working with Specialised Language: A Practical Guide to Using Corpora. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bus, A. G., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 121. doi: 00346543065001001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T. (2014). The interaction of gesture, intonation, and eye-gaze in proto-imperatives. Journal of Child Language, 41, 842860. doi: 10.1017/S0305000913000238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, i–174. doi: 10.2307/1166214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, C., & Luo, Y. (2020). A longitudinal study of maternal interaction strategies during joint book-reading in Taiwan. Journal of Child Language, 47, 401407. doi: 10.1017/S0305000919000746CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chouinard, M., & Clark, E. V. (2003). Adult reformulation of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language, 30, 637–69. doi: 10.1017/S0305000903005701CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chow, B. W.-Y., McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H., & Chow, C. S.-L. (2008). Dialogic reading and morphology training in Chinese children: Effects on language and literacy. Developmental Psychology, 44, 233244. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cline, K. D. (2010). ‘The Instructional and Emotional Quality of Parent–Child Book Reading and Early Head Start Children’s Learning Outcomes.’ Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Nebraska, Nebraska, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowe, L. K. (2000). Reading behaviors of mothers and their children with language impairment during repeated storybook reading. Journal of Communication Disorders, 33, 503524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curenton, S. M., Craig, M. J., & Flanigan, N. (2008). Use of decontextualized talk across story contexts: How oral storytelling and emergent reading can scaffold children’s development. Early Education & Development, 19, 161187. doi: 10.1080/10409280701839296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, B. (2011). The Storm Whale. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Dawson, N., Hasio, Y., Tan, N., Banerji, A. W. M., & Nation, K. A. (2021). Features of lexical richness in children’s books: Comparisons with child-directed speech. Language Development Research, 1, 9- 53. doi: 10.34842/5we1-yk94Google Scholar
Eckert, P., & McConnell-Ginet, S. (1992). ‘Think practically and look locally: language and gender as community-based practice’. Annual Review of Anthropology, 21, 461–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, P., & McConnell-Ginet, S. (1999). New generalizations and explanations in language and gender research. Language in Society, 28, 185201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ELAN (Version 5.2) [Computer software]. (2016). Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, The Language Archive. Retrieved from https://archive.mpi.nl/tla/elanGoogle Scholar
Elley, W. B. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 174187. doi: 10.2307/747863CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrant, B. M., & Zubrick, S. R. (2013). Parent-child book reading across early childhood and child vocabulary in the early school years: Findings from the longitudinal study of Australian children. First Language, 33, 280293. doi: 10.1177/0142723713487617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, K. L., Cross, J. R., Tanney, A. L., Schneider, M., & Finch, W. (2008). Predicting language development in children at risk: The effects of quality and frequency of caregiver reading. Early Education and Development, 19, 89111. doi: 10.1080/10409280701839106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleury, V. P., & Hugh, M. L. (2018). Exploring engagement in shared reading activities Between children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and their caregivers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48, 35963607. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3632-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgakopoulou, A. (1997). Narrative Performances. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hindman, A. H., Connor, C. M., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2008). Untangling the effects of shared book reading: Multiple factors and their associations with preschool literacy outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 330350. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.01.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26, 10711083. doi: 10.1177/0956797615581493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2000Enhancing children’s print and word awareness through home-based parent intervention. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 257269. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360.0903.257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2004). Print referencing: An emergent literacy enhancement strategy and its clinical applications. Language, Speech, & Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 185193. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2004/018)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, J. D. (1997). Gramática y corpus: los pronombres demostrativos. Revista de filología románica, 14, 291298. doi:Google Scholar
Koch, J. D. (2001). Un corpus informatizado para la enseñanza de la lengua española. Punto de partido y término. Hispanica Polonorum, 3, 6086.Google Scholar
Korat, O. (2009). The effect of maternal teaching talk on children’s emergent literacy as a function of type of activity and maternal education level. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 3442. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kucirkova, N., Messer, D., & Whitelock, V. (2013). Parents reading with their toddlers: The role of personalization in book engagement. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 13 ( 4), 445470. doi: 10.1177/1468798412438068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania PressGoogle Scholar
Laevers, F. (Ed.) (2008). Well-being and Involvement in Care Settings. A Process-oriented Self-evaluation Instrument (SiCs). Brussels: Kind & Gezin.Google Scholar
Laird, E. (2004). Beautiful Bananas. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lampropoulou, S. (2012). Direct Speech, Self-Presentation and Communitites of Practice. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Leech, K. A., Salo, V. C., Rowe, M. L., & Cabrera, N. J. (2013). Father input and child vocabulary development: The importance of wh questions and clarification requests. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34, 249259. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1353445Google ScholarPubMed
Lefebvre, P., Trudeau, N., & Sutton, A. (2011). Enhancing vocabulary, print awareness and phonological awareness through shared storybook reading with low-income pre-schoolers. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 11, 453479. doi: 10.1177/1468798411416581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, R., Hall, M., & Preece, J. (2018). Examining the links between parents’ relationships with reading and shared reading with their pre-school children. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 7 ( 2), 123150. doi:10.17583/ijep.2018.3480LCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lingwood, J., Billington, J., & Rowland, C. F. (2020a). Evaluating the effectiveness of a ‘real-world’ shared reading intervention for preschool children and their families: A randomised controlled trial. Journal of Reading Research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lingwood, J., Levy, R., Billington, J., & Rowland, C. F. (2020b). Barriers and solutions to family-based education interventions. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 23, 185198. doi: 10.1080/13645579.2019.1645377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mantei, J., & Kervin, L. (2014). Interpreting the images in a picture book: Students make connections to themselves, their lives and experiences. English Teaching: Practice and Critique, 13, 7692.Google Scholar
McCarthey, S. J. (1997). Connecting home and school literacy practices in classrooms with diverse populations. Journal of Literacy Research, 29, 145182. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862969709547955CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montag, J. L., Jones, M. N., & Smith, L. B. (2015). The words children hear: Picture books and the statistics for language learning. Psychological Science, 26, 14891496. doi: 10.1177/0956797615594361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moody, A. K., Justice, L. M., & Cabell, S. Q. (2010). Electronic versus traditional storybooks: Relative influence on preschool children’s engagement and communication. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 10(3), 294313. doi: 10.1177/1468798410372162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, L. M. (1988). Young children’s responses to one-to-one readings in school settings. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 89107. doi: 10.2307/747906CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murata, K. (1994). Intrusive or co-operative? A cross-cultural study of interruption. Journal of Pragmatics, 21(4), 385400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, C. H., Cameron-Faulkner, T., & Lieven, E. (2018). Keeping it simple: The grammatical properties of shared book reading. Journal of Child Language, 45, 753766. doi:10.1017/S0305000917000447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, C., Sala, G., Lowe, M., Lingwood, J., Rowland, C. F., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. (2019). The impact of shared book reading on children’s language skills: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 28, 110. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (2001). Living narrative: Creating lives in everyday storytelling. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ortiz, C., Stowe, R. M., & Arnold, D. H. (2001). Parental influence on child interest in shared picture book reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 16, 263281. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(01)00101-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pastor, G. C., & Seghiri, M. (2010). Size matters: A quantitative approach to corpus representativeness. Retrieved from: https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/224792152.pdfGoogle Scholar
Pine, J. M. (1991). Relationships between maternal speech characteristics and individual differences in early vocabulary composition. First Language, 11, 416417. doi:10.1177/014272379101103319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preece, J., & Levy, R. (2018). Understanding the barriers and motivations to shared reading with young children: The role of enjoyment and feedback, Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 20, 120. doi: 10.1177/1468798418779216Google Scholar
Price, L. H., Bradley, B. A., & Smith, J. M. (2012). A comparison of preschool teachers’ talk during storybook and information book read-alouds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 426440. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rathmann, P. (1994). Goodnight Gorilla by London: Egmont.Google Scholar
Rees, K., Nadig, A., & Rvachew, S. (2017). Story-related discourse by parent–child dyads: A comparison of typically developing children and children with language impairments. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 12, 1623. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2017.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E. (1995). Predicting children’s literacy from mother-child conversations. Cognitive Development, 10, 381405. doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(95)90003-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richter, A., & Courage, M. L. (2017). Comparing electronic and paper storybooks for preschoolers: Attention, engagement, and recall. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 48, 92102. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2017.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romeo, R. R., Leonard, J. A., Robinson, S. T., West, M. R., Mackey, A. P., Rowe, M. L., et al. (2018). Beyond the 30-million-word-gap: Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychological Science, 29(5), 700710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E., Arlman-Rupp, A., Hassing, Y., Jobse, J., Joosten, J., & Vorster, J. (1976). Mothers’ speech in three social classes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5, 120. doi: 10.1007/BF01067944CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C., & Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Son, S-H. C., & Tineo, M. F. (2016). Mothers’ attention-getting utterances during shared book reading: Links to low-income preschoolers’ verbal engagement, visual attention, and early literacy. Infant and Child Development, 25, 259282. doi: 10.1002/icd.1932CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D. (1983). I take out the Rock-Dok! How Greek women tell about being molested and create involvement. Anthropological Linguistics, 25, 359374.Google Scholar
Tannen, D. (1994). Gender and Discourse. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1992). Accelerating language development through picture book reading: A systematic extension to Mexican day care. Developmental Psychology, 28, 11061114. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of Practice: learning, meaning and identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., Falco, F. L., Lonigan, C., Fischel, J. E., DeBaryshe, B. D., Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., & Caulfield, M. (1988). Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Developmental Psychology, 24, 552558. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.24.4.552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wicks, R., Paynter, J., & Westerveld, M. F. (2020). Looking or talking: Visual attention and verbal engagement during shared book reading of preschool children on the autism spectrum. Autism, 24, 13841399. doi: 10.1177/136236131990059CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of sample at baseline. Numbers refer to mean (SD) for Children’s age, and N (%) for all other rows.

Figure 1

Table 2. Coding scheme used to measure turntaking, head direction, speech acts, and speech act responses during shared book reading4

Figure 2

Table 3. The Leuven Scale of Active Engagement, taken from Laevers (2008)

Figure 3

Table 4. Identifying which behaviours are associated with low and high engagement from the wider literature. Attentional directives have been characterised as both low and high engagement in the literature

Figure 4

Figure 1. The paired mean difference between low and high engagement is shown in a series of Gardner-Altman estimation plots below. Both groups are plotted on the X axes as a slopegraph: each paired set of observations is connected by a line. The paired mean difference is plotted on a floating axis on the right as a bootstrap sampling distribution. Mean difference is depicted as a dot. The 95% confidence interval is indicated by the ends of the vertical error bar.

Figure 5

Table 5. T-scores, P-values, and mean difference effect size for proportion of time spent in each language-boosting behaviour across moments of low and high engagement.

Figure 6

Appendix 1. Mean proportion of time in language boosting behaviours across engagement levels. Standard deviations are in parentheses.