Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T00:02:58.996Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attention to the mouth and gaze following in infancy predict language development*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2014

ELENA J. TENENBAUM*
Affiliation:
Brown Center for the Study of Children at Risk at Women and Infants Hospital Providence, RI
DAVID M. SOBEL
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI
STEPHEN J. SHEINKOPF
Affiliation:
Brown Center for the Study of Children at Risk at Women and Infants Hospital Providence, RI and Department of Psychiatry, Brown University, Providence, RI
BERTRAM F. MALLE
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI
JAMES L. MORGAN
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI
*
Address for correspondence: Elena Tenenbaum, Brown Center for the Study of Children at Risk, Women and Infants Hospital, 50 Holden St., Providence RI 02906. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We investigated longitudinal relations among gaze following and face scanning in infancy and later language development. At 12 months, infants watched videos of a woman describing an object while their passive viewing was measured with an eye-tracker. We examined the relation between infants' face scanning behavior and their tendency to follow the speaker's attentional shift to the object she was describing. We also collected language outcome measures on the same infants at 18 and 24 months. Attention to the mouth and gaze following at 12 months both predicted later productive vocabulary. The results are discussed in terms of social engagement, which may account for both attentional distribution and language onset. We argue that an infant's inherent interest in engaging with others (in addition to creating more opportunities for communication) leads infants to attend to the most relevant information in a social scene and that this information facilitates language learning.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by NIH grant HD32005 to JLM and a graduate research award from the Brain Sciences Program at Brown University to EJT and DMS, and an Autism Speaks postdoctoral fellowship (grant 7897) to EJT and SJS. Thanks go to Lori Rolfe for substantial effort in recruiting and testing, Halie Rando for her face, and the participant families for all of their efforts. We also thank Scott P. Johnson at UCLA for the attention attracting video used to calibrate the infants in this study.

References

REFERENCES

Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J. (eds), Joint attention: its origins and role in development, 131–58. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2002). Mindreading, communication and the learning of names for things. Mind & Language 17, 3754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L. & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one's age: vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112, 417–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science 8, 535–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, R. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: a longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language 35, 207–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child's talk: learning to use language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common is space: spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9, 5572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development 63(i–vi), 1174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chawarska, K., Macari, S. & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry 74, 195203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chawarska, K. & Shic, F. (2009). Looking but not seeing: atypical visual scanning and recognition of faces in 2 and 4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39, 1663–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology 52, 337–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corkum, V. & Moore, C. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J. (eds), Joint attention: its origins and role in development, 6184. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Corkum, V. & Moore, C. (1998). Origins of joint visual attention in infants. Developmental Psychology 34, 2838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: the case for pedagogy. Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and Performance XXI(21), 249274.Google Scholar
D'Entremont, B. (2000). A perceptual-attentional explanation of gaze following in 3 and 6 month olds. Developmental Science 3, 302–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Entremont, B., Hains, S. M. J. & Muir, D. W. (1997). A demonstration of gaze following in 3- to 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development 20, 569–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falck-Ytter, T., Fernell, E., Gillberg, C. & Von Hofsten, C. (2010). Face scanning distinguishes social from communication impairments in autism. Developmental Science 13, 864–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J. & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (Vol. 59).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S. & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories – User's Guide and Technical Manual, 2nd ed.Baltimore: Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Gredebäck, G., Fikke, L. & Melinder, A. (2010). The development of joint visual attention: a longitudinal study of gaze following during interactions with mothers and strangers. Developmental Science 13, 839–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewkowicz, D. J. & Hansen-Tift, A. M. (2012). Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109, 1431–1436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1996). Why do infants begin to talk? Language as an unintended consequence. Journal of Child Language 23, 251–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A. & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science 11, F9F16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E. F., Yale, M., Messinger, D., Neal, R. & Schwartz, H. K. (2000a). Responding to joint attention across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 21, 283–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E. F., Yale, M., Neal, R. & Schwartz, H. K. (2000b). Gaze following, temperament, and language development in 6-month-olds: a replication and extension. Infant Behavior and Development 23, 231–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, M., Mundy, P. & Rojas, J. (1998). Following the direction of gaze and language development in 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development 21, 373–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P. & Gomes, A. (1998). Individual differences in joint attention skill development in the second year. Infant Behavior and Development 21, 469–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Kasari, C., Sigman, M. & Ruskin, E. (1995). Nonverbal communication and early language acquisition in children with Down Syndrome and in normally developing children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 157–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perra, O. & Gattis, M. (2010). The control of social attention from 1 to 4 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 28, 891908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, J. E., Reynolds, G. D. & Courage, M. L. (2010). The neural bases of infant attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19, 41–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scaife, M. & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature 253, 265–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senju, A. & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology 18, 668–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheinkopf, S. J., Mundy, P., Claussen, A. H. & Willoughby, J. (2004). Infant joint attention skill and preschool behavioral outcomes in at-risk children. Developmental Psychopathology 16, 273–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shic, F., Bradshaw, J., Klin, A., Scassellati, B. & Chawarska, K. (2011). Limited activity monitoring in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research 1380, 246–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shic, F., Macari, S. & Chawarska, K. (2013). Speech disturbs face scanning in 6-month-old infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychiatry.Google ScholarPubMed
Tenenbaum, E., Amso, D., Abar, B. & Sheinkopf, S. J. (2014). Attention and word learning in autistic, language delayed and typically developing children. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tenenbaum, E., Shah, R. J., Sobel, D. M., Malle, B. F. & Morgan, J. L. (2013). Increased focus on the mouth among infants in the first year of life: a longitudinal eye-tracking study. Infancy 18, 534–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57, 14541463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevarthen, C. & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 42, 348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughan Van Hecke, A., Mundy, P. C., Acra, C. F., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E. F., Parlade, M. V., & Pomares, Y. B. (2007). Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. Child Development 78, 5369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
YoungG., S. G., S. , Merin, N., Rogers, S. J. & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science 12, 798814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed