Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T13:26:15.057Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adjective forms and functions in British English child-directed speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 June 2019

Catherine DAVIES
Affiliation:
University of Leeds, UK
Jamie LINGWOOD*
Affiliation:
University of Leeds, UK
Sudha ARUNACHALAM
Affiliation:
New York University, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Dr Jamie Lingwood, School of Languages, Cultures and Societies, University of Leeds, LS2 9JT, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Adjectives are essential for describing and differentiating concepts. However, they have a protracted development relative to other word classes. Here we measure three- and four-year-olds’ exposure to adjectives across a range of interactive and socioeconomic contexts to: (i) measure the syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic variability of adjectives in child-directed speech (CDS); and (ii) investigate how features of the input might scaffold adjective acquisition. In our novel corpus of UK English, adjectives occurred more frequently in prenominal than in postnominal (predicative) syntactic frames, though postnominal frames were more frequent for less-familiar adjectives. They occurred much more frequently with a descriptive than a contrastive function, especially for less-familiar adjectives. Our findings present a partial mismatch between the forms of adjectives found in real-world CDS and those forms that have been shown to be more useful for learning. We discuss implications for models of adjective acquisition and for clinical practice.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing Noun-Phrase antecedents. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Arunachalam, S. (2016). A new experimental paradigm to study children's processing of their parent's unscripted language input, Journal of Memory and Language, 88, 104–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, T. K., & Laframboise, D. E. (1990). Acquiring color names via linguistic contrast: the influence of contrastive terms. Child Development, 61, 1808–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, T. K., & Markman, E. M. (1987). Acquiring word meanings via linguistic contrast. Cognitive Development, 2, 217–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, E. J. (1976). Sizing things up: the acquisition of the meaning of dimensional adjectives. Journal of Child Language, 3, 205–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baxter, J., & Hayes, A. (2007). How four year-olds spend their day: insights into the caring contexts of young children. Family Matters, 76, 3443. Retrieved from <https://aifs.gov.au/sites/default/files/jb%282%29.pdf>.Google Scholar
Behrens, H. (2006). The input–output relationship in first language acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. (1988). On the ability to relate events in narrative. Discourse Processes, 11, 469–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, A. A. (2005). Acquiring the English adjective lexicon: relationships with input properties and adjectival semantic typology. Journal of Child Language, 32, 535–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Hendricks, C., Haynes, O. M., & Painter, K. M. (2007). Maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness: associations with social context, maternal characteristics, and child characteristics in a multivariate analysis. Infancy, 12, 189223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. (1958). Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 1221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Biemiller, A. (2017). Test-based age-of-acquisition norms for 44 thousand English word meanings. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 1520–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, N. (2011). One snowy night. London: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., & Noble, C. (2013). A comparison of book text and child directed speech. First Language, 33, 268–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference, 15, 1729.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, J., Fenson, L., Sanderl, L., & Weir, J. (1995). A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development, 10, 159–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In Li, C. N. (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 2556). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. (1994). Discourse, consciousness, and time. Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1972). On the child's acquisition of antonyms in two semantic fields. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 750–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2009). First language acquisition (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociology, 94, S95S120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conger, R. D., & Donnellan, M. B. (2007). An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 175–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crain-Thoreson, C., Dahlin, M. P., & Powell, T. A. (2001). Parent–child interaction in three conversational contexts: variations in style and strategy. In Britto, P. R. & Brooks-Gunn, J. (Eds.), Role of family literacy environments in promoting young children's emerging literacy skills (pp. 723). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S., & Fenson, L. (1996). Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 125–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, C. & Katsos, N. (2010). Over-informative children: production/comprehension asymmetry or tolerance to pragmatic violations? Lingua, 120, 1956–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, C., & Kreysa, H. (2018) Look before you speak: children's integration of visual information into informative referring expressions. Journal of Child Language, 45, 1116–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department for Communities and Local Government (2015). English indices of deprivation. Retrieved from <https://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/english-indices-of-deprivation-2015>..>Google Scholar
Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T. (1982). Social-interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition, 11, 159–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Earle, C., & Lowry, L. (2011). Making Hanen Happen: leader's guide for Target Word™ — The Hanen Program® for Parents of Children Who Are Late Talkers (3rd ed.) Toronto: Hanen Early Language Program.Google Scholar
Ebeling, K. S., & Gelman, S. A. (1994). Children's use of context in interpreting ‘big’ and ‘little’. Child Development, 65, 1178–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ensminger, M. E., & Fothergill, K. E. (2003). A decade of measuring SES: what it tells us and where to go from here. In Bornstein, M. H. & Bradley, R. H. (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 1327). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Thorpe, K., & Marchman, V. A. (2010). Blue car, red car: developing efficiency in online interpretation of adjective–noun phrases. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 190217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gasser, M., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Learning nouns and adjectives: a connectionist account. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 269306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gatt, A., Krahmer, E., Deemter, K. van, & van Gompel, R. P. G. (2017). Reference production as search: the impact of domain size on the production of distinguishing descriptions. Cognitive Science, 41, 1457–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: linguistic relativity vs. natural partitioning. In Kuczaj, S. A. (Ed.), Language development. Vol 2: language, thought and culture (pp. 301–34). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relativity and early word learning. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 215–56). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Pearce, P. S. (1999). The relationship between maternal language measures and language development in toddlers with expressive vocabulary delays. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 364–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions. Language, 69, 274307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
He, A. X., & Arunachalam, S. (2017). Word learning mechanisms. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 8, e1435. doi:10.1002/wcs.1435Google ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 1368–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofferth, S. L., & Sandberg, J. F. (2001). How American children spend their time. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 295308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 782–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1994). Influences of mother and child on maternal talkativeness. Discourse Processes, 18, 105–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y., & Snedeker, J. (2013). The use of referential context in children's on-line interpretation of scalar adjectives. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1090–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson-Kam, C., & Matthewson, L. (2017). Introducing the Infant Bookreading Database (IBDb). Journal of Child Language, 44, 1289–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H. R., Vevea, J. L., & Hedges, L. V. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1062–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kennedy, C., & McNally, L. (2005). Scale structure, degree modification, and the semantics of gradable predicates. Language, 81, 345–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, C., & McNally, L. (2010). Color, context, and compositionality. Synthese, 174, 7998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klibanoff, R. S., & Waxman, S. R. (2000). Basic level object categories support the acquisition of novel adjectives: evidence from preschool-aged children. Child Development, 71, 649–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klinger, J., Mayor, J., & Bannard, C. (2016). Children's faithfulness in imitating language use varies cross-culturally, contingent on prior experience. Child Development, 87, 820–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knowles, W., & Masidlover, M. (1982) The Derbyshire Language Scheme. Derby: Derbyshire County Council.Google Scholar
Kronmüller, E., Morisseau, T., & Noveck, I. A. (2014). Show me the pragmatic contribution: a developmental investigation of contrastive inference. Journal of Child Language, 41, 9851014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, V., & Shipley, E. F. (1996). Parental speech to middle and working class children from two racial groups in three settings. Applied Psycholinguistics 17, 233–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, A., Ginsborg, J., & Peers, I. (2002) Development and disadvantage: implications for the early years and beyond. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Markman, E. M. (1990). Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science, 14, 5777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Freedman, M. L. (2001). Short-term retention of lexical–semantic representations: implications for speech production. Memory, 9(4/6), 261–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: effects of distractors and feedback on referential communication. Topics in Cognitive Science 4, 184210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: a training study. Child Development, 78, 1744–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montag, J. L., Jones, M. N., & Smith, L. B. (2015). The words children hear: picture books and the statistics for language learning. Psychological Science, 26, 1489–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, M. L., & Jones, S. (2008). Antonyms in children's and child directed speech. First Language, 28, 403–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L. R., & Hoff-Ginsburg, E. (1998) Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. Journal of Child Language, 25, 95120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition (pp. 109–49). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (1988). On formal grammatical categories in early child language. In Levy, Y., Schlesinger, I. M., & Braine, M. D. S. (Eds.), Categories and processes in language acquisition (pp. 99119). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (2004). Young children's difficulty with adjectives modifying nouns. Journal of Child Language, 31, 255–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, C. H., Cameron-Faulkner, T., & Lieven, E. (2017). Keeping it simple: the grammatical properties of shared book reading. Journal of Child Language, 45, 114.Google ScholarPubMed
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Journal of Child Development, 76, 763–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Pechmann, T. (1984). Accentuation and redundancy in children and adult's referential communication. In Bouma, H. & Bouwhuis, D. G. (Eds), Attention and performance: control of language processes (pp. 417–31). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Thorpe, K., & Denny, K. (2007). Surprise in the learning of color words. Proceedings of the 29th Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Nashville, TN. Retrieved from: <https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/307c/98bf52f3ccb8ac5ce656d5e6ea545c6c2228.pdf>..>Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Yarlett, D., Dye, M., Denny, K., & Thorpe, K. (2010) The effects of feature-label-order and their implications for symbolic learning. Cognitive Science, 34, 909–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, I. S., & Bryant, P. (1990). Social background, phonological awareness and children's reading. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 209–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ricks, S. L., & Alt, M. (2016). Theoretical principles to guide the teaching of adjectives to children who struggle with word learning: synthesis of experimental and naturalistic research with principles of learning theory. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 47, 181–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RStudio Team (2018). RStudio: integrated development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc. Online <http://www.rstudio.com/>..>Google Scholar
Salerni, N., Assanelli, A., D'Odorico, L., & Rossi, G. (2007). Qualitative aspects of productive vocabulary at the 200- and 500-word stages: a comparison between spontaneous speech and parental report data. First Language, 27, 7587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandhofer, C. M., & Smith, L. B. (2007). Learning adjectives in the real world: how learning nouns impedes learning adjectives. Language, Learning and Development, 3, 233–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandhofer, C. M., Smith, L. B., & Luo, J. (2000). Counting nouns and verbs in the input: Differential frequencies, different kinds of learning? Journal of Child Language, 27, 561–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulze, C., Grassmann., S., & Tomasello, M. (2013). 3-year-old children make relevance inferences in indirect verbal communication. Child Development, 84, 2079–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sedivy, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., Chambers, C. G., & Carlson, G. N. (1999). Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71, 109–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sekerina, I. A., & Trueswell, J. C. (2012). Interactive processing of contrastive expressions by Russian children. First Language, 32, 6387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43, 549–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soderstrom, M., & Wittebolle, K. (2013). When do caregivers talk? The influences of activity and time of day on caregiver speech and child vocalizations in two childcare environments. PloS one, 8, e80646. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080646CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Syrett, K., Kennedy, C., & Lidz, J. (2010). Meaning and context in children's understanding of gradable adjectives. Journal of Semantics, 27, 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tardif, T., Shatz, M., & Naigles, L. (1997). Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs: a comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language, 24(3), 535–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tare, M., Shatz, M., & Gilbertson, L. (2008). Maternal uses of non-object terms in child-directed speech: color, number and time. First Language, 28, 87100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, K., & Fernald, A. (2006). Knowing what a novel word is not: two-year-olds ‘listen through’ ambiguous adjectives in fluent speech. Cognition, 100, 389433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Stahl, D. (2004). Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language, 31, 101–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tommerdahl, J., & Kilpatrick, C. (2013). Analyzing reliability of grammatical production in spontaneous samples of varying length. Journal of Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 29, 171–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E. (2009). Reference points in linguistic construal: scalar adjectives revisited. Studia Linguistica 63, 233–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E. (2011a). Reference points in adjective–noun conceptual integration networks. In Handl, S. & Schmid, H. J. (Eds.), Windows to the mind: metaphor, metonymy and conceptual blending (pp. 269–90). Berlin & New York: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Tribushinina, E. (2011b). Once again on norms and comparison classes. Linguistics, 49, 525–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E. (2012). Comprehension of relevance implicatures by pre-schoolers: the case of adjectives. Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 2035–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E. (2013a). Adjective semantics, world knowledge and visual context: comprehension of size terms by 2- to 7-year-old Dutch-speaking children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42, 205–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E. (2013b). Spatial adjectives in Dutch child language: towards a usage-based model of adjective acquisition. In Paradis, C., Hudson, J., & Magnusson, U., (Eds.), Conceptual spaces and the construal of spatial meaning: empirical evidence from human communication (pp. 263–86). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E., & Gillis, S. (2012). The acquisition of scalar structures: production of adjectives and degree markers by Dutch-speaking children and their caregivers. Linguistics, 50, 241–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E., & Mak, W. (2016). Three-year-olds can predict a noun based on an attributive adjective: evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Child Language, 43(2), 425–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tribushinina, E., Mak, M., Dubinkina, E., & Mak, W. M. (2018). Adjective production by Russian-speaking children with developmental language disorder and Dutch–Russian simultaneous bilinguals: disentangling the profiles. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(5), 1033–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E., van den Bergh, H., Kilani-Schoch, M., Aksu-Koç, A., Dabašinskienė, I., Hrzica, G., Korecky-Kröll, K., Noccetti, S., & Dressler, W. (2013). The role of explicit contrast in adjective acquisition: a cross-linguistic longitudinal study of adjective production in spontaneous child speech and parental input. First Language, 33, 594616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tribushinina, E., van den Bergh, H., Ravid, D., Aksu-Koç, A., Kilani-Schoch, M, Korecky-Kröll, K., Leibovitch-Cohen, I., Laaha, S., Nir, B., Dressler, W. U., & Gillis, S. (2014). Development of adjective frequencies across semantic classes: a growth curve analysis of child speech and child-directed speech. LIA Language, Interaction and Acquisition, 5(2), 185226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voeikova, M. D. (2003). Tipy i raznovidnosti kvalitativnyx otnošenij na rannix etapax rečevogo razvitija rebenka: Analiz reči vzroslogo, obrašennoj k rebenku [Types and subtypes of qualitative relations in the early stages of speech development: analysis of the input]. In Bondarko, A. V. & Shubik, S. A. (Eds.), Problemy funkcional'noj grammatiki: Semantičeskaja invariantnost’/ variativnost’ [Issues in functional grammar: semantic invariency/variency] (pp. 206–35). St. Petersburg: Nauka.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R., & Booth, A. E. (2001). On the insufficiency of evidence for a domain-general account of word learning. Cognition, 78, 277–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waxman, S. R., & Klibanoff, R. (2000). The role of comparison in the acquisition of novel adjectives. Cognition 70, 3550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J. (1997). Language processes in context: language learning in children reared in poverty. In Adamson, L. B. & Romski, M. A. (Eds.), Research on communication and language disorders: contribution to theories of language development (pp. 233–66). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Vo, M. L.-H., Evans, K. K., & Greene, M. R. (2011). Visual search in scenes involves selective and nonselective pathways. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(2), 7784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshida, H., & Hanania, R. (2013). If it's red, it's not Vap: how competition among words may benefit early word learning. First Language, 33, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar