Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:15:03.578Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Acquisition of weak syllables in tonal languages: acoustic evidence from neutral tone in Mandarin Chinese

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 August 2018

Ping TANG*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney
Ivan YUEN
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney
Nan XU RATTANASONE
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney
Liqun GAO*
Affiliation:
School of Communication Science, Beijing Language and Culture University, Beijing
Katherine DEMUTH
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney
*
*Corresponding authors: Ping Tang, Department of Linguistics, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, 16 University Avenue, Australian Hearing Hub, Balaclava Road, North Ryde, NSW 2109Australia E-mail: [email protected]. Liqun Gao, School of Communication Science, Beijing Language and Culture University, 15 Xueyuan Road, Haidain District, 100083, Beijing, China. E-mail: [email protected]
*Corresponding authors: Ping Tang, Department of Linguistics, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, 16 University Avenue, Australian Hearing Hub, Balaclava Road, North Ryde, NSW 2109Australia E-mail: [email protected]. Liqun Gao, School of Communication Science, Beijing Language and Culture University, 15 Xueyuan Road, Haidain District, 100083, Beijing, China. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Weak syllables in Germanic and Romance languages have been reported to be challenging for young children, with syllable omission and/or incomplete reduction persisting till age five. In Mandarin Chinese, neutral tone (T0) involves a weak syllable with varied pitch realizations across (preceding) tonal contexts and short duration. The present study examined how and when T0 was acquired by 108 Beijing Mandarin-speaking children (3–5 years) relative to 33 adult controls. Lexicalized (familiar) and non-lexicalized (unfamiliar) T0 words were elicited in different preceding tonal contexts. Unlike previous reports, the present study revealed that children as young as three years have already developed a phonological category for T0, exhibiting contextually conditioned tonal realizations of T0 for both familiar and unfamiliar items. However, mastery of adult-like pitch and duration implementation of T0 is a protracted process not completed until age five. The implications for the acquisition of weak syllables more generally are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–78.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version, 1(7), 123. Retrieved from <https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4>..>Google Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word, 14(2/3), 150–77.Google Scholar
Boersma, P. (1993). Accurate short-term analysis of the fundamental frequency and the harmonics-to-noise ratio of a sampled sound. In Proceedings of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences, 17(1193), 97110.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2016). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0. 19. Retrieved from <www.praat.org>..>Google Scholar
Cao, J. (1992). On neutral-tone syllables in Mandarin Chinese. Canadian Acoustics, 20(3), 4950.Google Scholar
Chang, C. (1998). The development of autonomy in preschool Mandarin Chinese-speaking children's play narratives. Narrative Inquiry, 8(1), 77111.Google Scholar
Chen, Y., & Xu, Y. (2006). Production of weak elements in speech–evidence from f₀ patterns of neutral tone in Standard Chinese. Phonetica, 63(1), 4775.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (1996). The prosodic structure of early words. In Morgan, J. & Demuth, K. (Eds.), Signal to syntax: bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 171–84). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Demuth, K., & Johnson, M. (2003). Truncation to subminimal words in early French. Canadian Journal of Linguistics / Revue canadienne de linguistique, 48(3/4), 211–41.Google Scholar
Deng, X., & Yip, V. (2018). A multimedia corpus of child Mandarin: the Tong corpus. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 46(1), 6992.Google Scholar
Fikkert, P. (1993). The acquisition of Dutch stress. In Verrips, M. & Wijnen, F. (Eds.), Amsterdam Series in Child Language Development, Vol. 1 (pp. 2135). Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam, Instituut voor Algemene Taalwetenschap.Google Scholar
Fry, D. B. (1955). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27(4), 765–68.Google Scholar
Gerken, L. (1994). A metrical template account of children's weak syllable omissions from multisyllabic words. Journal of Child Language, 21(3), 565–84.Google Scholar
Haelsig, P. C., & Madison, C. L. (1986). A study of phonological processes exhibited by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 17(2), 107–14.Google Scholar
Hua, Z. (2002). Phonological development in specific contexts: studies of Chinese-speaking children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Hua, Z., & Dodd, B. (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (Modern Standard Chinese). Journal of Child Language, 27(1), 342.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1974). Phonological rules in young children. Journal of Child Language, 1(1), 4964.Google Scholar
Kehoe, M., & Lleó, C. (2003). A phonological analysis of schwa in German first language acquisition. Canadian Journal of Linguistics / Revue canadienne de linguistique, 48(3/4), 289327.Google Scholar
Kehoe, M., Stoel-Gammon, C., & Buder, E. H. (1995). Acoustic correlates of stress in young children's speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38(2), 338–50.Google Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2015). Package ‘lmerTest’. R package version, 2(0). Retrieved from <https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest>..>Google Scholar
Lenth, R. V. (2016). Least-squares means: the R package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software, 69(1), 133.Google Scholar
Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1977). The acquisition of tone in Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 4(2), 185–99.Google Scholar
Lleó, C. (2006). The acquisition of prosodic word structures in Spanish by monolingual and Spanish–German bilingual children. Language and Speech, 49(2), 205–29.Google Scholar
Luke, S. G. (2017). Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R. Behavior Research Methods, 49(4), 1494–502.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk, 3rd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
Oller, K., & Eilers, R. E. (1975). Phonetic expectation and transcription validity. Phonetica, 31(3/4), 288304.Google Scholar
Peters, J., Hanssen, J., & Gussenhoven, C. (2014). The phonetic realization of focus in West Frisian, Low Saxon, High German, and three varieties of Dutch. Journal of Phonetics, 46, 185209.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer program]. Version 3.3.1. Retrieved from < https://www.R-project.org/> (last accessed 21 June 2016).+(last+accessed+21+June+2016).>Google Scholar
Shen, X. S. (1992). Mandarin neutral tone revisited. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia, 24(1), 131–52.Google Scholar
Tang, P. (2014). A study of prosodic errors of Chinese neutral tone by advanced Japanese students (Chinese version). TCSOL Studies, 56(4), 3947.Google Scholar
Tang, P., Xu Rattanasone, N., Yuen, I., & Demuth, K. (2017a). Phonetic enhancement of Mandarin vowels and tones: infant-directed speech and Lombard speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 142(2), 493503.Google Scholar
Tang, P., Xu Rattanasone, N., Yuen, I., & Demuth, K. (2017b). Acoustic realization of Mandarin neutral tone and tone sandhi in infant-directed speech and Lombard speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 142(5), 2823–35.Google Scholar
Xu, Y. (1997). Contextual tonal variations in Mandarin. Journal of Phonetics, 25(1), 6183.Google Scholar
Yip, M. (1989). Contour tones. Phonology, 6(1), 149–74.Google Scholar
Yip, M. (2002). Tone. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Yuen, I., Demuth, K., & Johnson, M. (2011). Prosodic structure in child speech planning and production. In Proceedings from the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 2248–51).Google Scholar