Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:56:45.721Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The acquisition of polysynthesis*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Marianne Mithun*
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Barbara
*
Department of Linguistics, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA

Abstract

Polysynthetic languages can present special extraction puzzles to children, due to the length of their words. A number of hypotheses concerning children's strategies for acquiring morphology, originally proposed on the basis of their approaches to somewhat simpler systems, are confirmed by observations of five children acquiring Mohawk. Among the Mohawk children, the earliest segmentation of words was phonological rather than morphological: stressed syllables, usually penultimate or antepenultimate, were extracted first. Ultimate syllables were then added, confirming the salience of the ends of words. During this time, distinctions expressed by adults in affixes were either omitted or expressed analytically. Acquisition then moved leftward by syllables. When most utterances were long enough to include pronominal prefixes as well as roots, morphological structure was apparently discovered. It is not surprising that the pronouns should trigger this awareness, since they are frequent, appearing with every verb and most nouns, they are functional, and they are semantically transparent. From this point on, the children acquired affixes primarily according to their utility and semantic transparency rather than their phonological shape or position.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

I am especially grateful to Mary MacDonald of Akwesasne, New York, for her help in interviewing two of the children described here, and for her comments on the resulting Mohawk language data. I also appreciate the help and expertise provided by Hilda Gabriel, Madeleine Montour and Minnie Nelson of Oka, Quebec, Leatrice Beauvais, Mary Cross, Annie Deer, Georgina Jacobs and Verna Jacobs of Caughnawaga, Quebec, Helen Thomas, Marie Sunday and Mildred White of Akwesasne, who assisted in interviewing the other children and discussing their language. Finally, I much appreciate the comments made by Ann Peters on an earlier version of this paper.

References

REFERENCES

Feuer, H. (1980). Morphological development in Mohawk. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 18. 2542.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1985). Hungarian language acquisition as an exemplification of a general model of grammatical development. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 2. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mithun, M. (1984). The evolution of noun incorporation. Language 60. 847–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithun, M. (1987). Is basic word order universal? In Tomlin, R. (ed.), Coherence and grounding in discourse. Typological Studies in Language 11. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Peters, Ann M. (1981). Language typology and the segmentation problem in early child language acquisition. In D. Alford et al. (eds), Proceedings of the Seventh Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Peters, Ann M. (1983). The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Peters, Ann M. (1985). Language segmentation: operating principles for the perception and analysts of language. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 2. Theoretical issues. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pye, Clifton. (1983). Mayan telegraphese. Language 59. 583604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. and Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar