Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T17:17:57.107Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What's in the input? Frequent frames in child-directed speech offer distributional cues to grammatical categories in Spanish and English*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 August 2009

ADRIANA WEISLEDER
Affiliation:
Stanford University
SANDRA R. WAXMAN*
Affiliation:
Northwestern University
*
Address for correspondence: Sandra R. Waxman, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL 60208-2710; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Recent analyses have revealed that child-directed speech contains distributional regularities that could, in principle, support young children's discovery of distinct grammatical categories (noun, verb, adjective). In particular, a distributional unit known as the frequent frame appears to be especially informative (Mintz, 2003). However, analyses have focused almost exclusively on the distributional information available in English. Because languages differ considerably in how the grammatical forms are marked within utterances, the scarcity of cross-linguistic evidence represents an unfortunate gap. We therefore advance the developmental evidence by analyzing the distributional information available in frequent frames across two languages (Spanish and English), across sentence positions (phrase medial and phrase final), and across grammatical forms (noun, verb, adjective). We selected six parent–child corpora from the CHILDES database (three English; three Spanish), and analyzed the input when children were aged 2 ; 6 or younger. In each language, frequent frames did indeed offer systematic cues to grammatical category assignment. We also identify differences in the accuracy of these frames across languages, sentences positions and grammatical classes.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by NIH R01 HD30410 to Waxman. Portions of this research were presented at the meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development (2007). We are indebted to E. Leddon for extensive discussions, and to M. Kaufman and T. Piccin for methodological expertise.

References

REFERENCES

Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, T. A. & Brent, M. R. (1997). Syntactic categorization in early language acquisition: Formalizing the role of distributional analysis. Cognition 63(2), 121–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chemla, E., Mintz, T. H., Bernal, S. & Christophe, A. (in press). Categorizing words using frequent frames: What cross-linguistic analyses reveal about distributional acquisition strategies. Developmental Science.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Boston, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & McRoberts, G. (1993). Effects of prosody and word position on infants' lexical comprehension. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27(2), 209221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A. & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers' speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology 20, 104113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiser, J. & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99(24), 15822–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, L. R. & Wanner, E. (1982). Language acquisition: The state of the art. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: The state of the art, 348. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gómez, R. L. (2002). Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science 13(5), 431–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gómez, R. L. & Maye, J. (2005). The developmental trajectory of nonadjacent dependency learning. Infancy 7(2), 183206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, Z. S. (1951). Methods in structural linguistics. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Jusczyk, P. W., Wright Cassidy, K., Druss, B. & Kennedy, L. (1987). Clauses are perceptual units for young infants. Cognition 26(3), 269–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiss, G. R. (1973). Grammatical word classes: A learning process and its simulation. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 7, 141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López-Ornat, S., Fernández, A., Gallo, P. & Mariscal, S. (1994). La Adquisición de la Lengua Española [The Acquisition of the Spanish Language], Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for anlyzing talk, 3rd edn, vol. 2: The Database. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. (1988). The acquisition of formal word classes. In Levy, Y., Schlesinger, I. M. & Braine, M. D. S. (eds), Categories and processes in language acquisition, 2144. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. & Chalkley, M. A. (1980). The internal language of children's syntax: The ontogenesis and representation of syntactic categories. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language, vol. 2, 127214. New York: Gardner.Google Scholar
Mintz, T. H. (2002). Category induction from distributional cues in an artificial language. Memory and Cognition 30, 678–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mintz, T. H. (2003). Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition 90(1), 91117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mintz, T. H. (2008). Distributional analysis as a method for categorizing words in infancy. Paper presented at the International Conference for Infant Studies, Vancouver, Canada.Google Scholar
Mintz, T. H., Newport, E. L. & Bever, T. G. (2002). The distributional structure of grammatical categories in speech to young children. Cognitive Science 26(4), 393424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montes, R. (1987). Secuencias de clarificación en conversaciones con niños [Clarification sequences in conversations with children]. Morphe 3–4: Universidad Autónoma de Puebla.Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L. & Newport, E. L. (1981). The role of constituent structure in the induction of an artificial language. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20, 6785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ojea, A. (2000). Llinàs-Ojea corpus: Irene. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The CHILDES database.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1987). The bootstrapping problem in language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition, 399442. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Redington, M., Chater, N. & Finch, S. (1998). Distributional information: A powerful cue for acquiring syntactic categories. Cognitive Science 22(4), 425–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent–child discourse. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language, 128. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Shady, M. & Gerken, L. (1999). Grammatical and caregiver cues in early sentence comprehension. Journal of Child Language 2, 163–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shi, R., Werker, J. F. & Morgan, J. L. (1999). Newborn infants' sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words. Cognition 72, B11B21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the acquisition of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development, 175208. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Snyder, W., Senghas, A. & Inman, K. (2001). Agreement morphology and the acquisition of Noun-drop in Spanish. Language Acquisition 9(2), 157–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suppes, P. (1974). The semantics of children's language. American Psychologist 29, 103114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrego, E. (1987). Empty categories in nominals. Unpublished manuscript, University of Massachusets, Boston.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R. & Booth, A. E. (2003). The origins and evolution of links between word learning and conceptual organization: New evidence from 11-month-olds. Developmental Science 6(2), 130–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waxman, S. R., Braun, I. E. & Weisleder, A. (in prep) The breadth of adjective learning in English- and Spanish-acquiring infants.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R. & Guasti, M. T. (in press). Linking nouns and adjectives to meaning: New evidence from Italian-speaking children. Language Learning and Development.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R. & Lidz, J. (2006). Early word learning. In Kuhn, D. & Siegler, R. (eds), Handbook of child psychology, 6th edn, vol. 2, 299335. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R., Senghas, A. & Benveniste, S. (1997). A cross-linguistic examination of the noun-category bias: Its existence and specificity in French- and Spanish-speaking preschool-aged children. Cognitive Psychology 32(3), 183218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed