Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T00:10:19.799Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Understanding different sources of information: the acquisition of evidentiality*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 September 2014

LOES KORING*
Affiliation:
Utrecht University, the Netherlands
HANNAH DE MULDER
Affiliation:
Utrecht University, the Netherlands
*
Address for correspondence: Loes Koring, Macquarie University, Department of Cognitive Science, Australian Hearing Hub, 16 University Avenue, NSW 2109, Australia. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This paper investigates six- to nine-year-old children's acquisition of evidentiality. In two minimally different tasks we assess whether children can be made to use a particular source of information by presenting them with a specific evidential term. That is, we assess whether children have an explicit awareness of the source requirement of the evidential terms. The results demonstrate that children explicitly understand the direct evidential term, but not the indirect evidential terms. Interestingly, the direct evidential term tested (Dutch lijken) does not encode high speaker certainty. Hence, even though the child cannot rely on speaker certainty to provide an answer, the results still show that direct evidentiality is acquired before indirect evidentiality.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The research reported was made possible by a grant from the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO, Nr. 021.001.087) to L. K. We would like to thank the children and teachers from Montessorischool Arcade and Montessori BS Houten (Vikingenpoort) for their participation. In addition, we would like to thank Iris Mulders and Theo Veenker for their help in setting up the experiment. Further, many thanks go to Andrea Gualmini for his help in creating the design of the experiment. We would also like to thank Pim Mak, Eric Reuland, Nina Hyams, Sharon Unsworth, and Sergio Baauw for their comments on earlier versions of this paper, Mattis van den Bergh and Huub van den Bergh for their help on the statistical analyses, and Lea ter Meulen for her help in coding the data. Finally, we would like to thank the audiences of On Linguistic Interfaces, Language Culture and Mind IV, the participants of the UCLA acquisition of semantics seminar, and the acquisition lab meeting in Groningen for useful comments.

References

REFERENCES

Aikhenvald, A. (2004). Evidentiality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. (1988). The acquisition of aspect and modality: the case of past reference in Turkish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A., Ögel-Balaban, H. & Alp, I. E. (2009). Evidentials and source knowledge in Turkish. In Fitneva, S. A. & Matsui, T. (eds), Evidentiality: a window into language and cognitive development (New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 125) (pp. 1328). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Becker, M. (2006). There began to be a learnability puzzle. Linguistic Inquiry 37, 441–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, C., Potts, C. & Speas, P. (2007). The pragmatic values of evidential sentences. In Gibson, M. & Friedman, T. (eds), Proceedings of SALT 17 (pp. 7188). New York: Cornell Linguistics Circle Publications.Google Scholar
De Haan, F. (1999). Evidentiality in Dutch. In Chang, S., Liaw, L. & Ruppenhofer, J. (eds), Proceedings of the Twenty-fifth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 74–85). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
De Haan, F. (2007). Raising as grammaticalization: the case of Germanic SEEM verbs. Rivista de Linguistica 19, 129–50.Google Scholar
Faller, M. (2002). Evidentiality in Cuzco Quechua. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Faller, M. (2006). Evidentiality above and below speech acts. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Manchester.Google Scholar
Faller, M. (2011). A possible worlds semantics for Cuzco Quechua evidential. In Li, Nan & Lutz, D. (eds), Proceedings of SALT 20 (pp. 660683). New York: Cornell Linguistics Circle Publications.Google Scholar
Fitneva, S. A. (2008). The role of evidentiality in Bulgarian children's reliability judgments. Journal of Child Language 35, 845–68.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. & Koenig, M. A. (2006). Trust in testimony: how children learn about science and religion. Child Development 77, 505–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsch, C., Orfitelli, R. & Wexler, K. (2008). The acquisition of raising reconsidered. In Gavarró, A. & Freitas, M. J. (eds), Language acquisition and development: proceedings of GALA 2007 (pp. 252261). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Press.Google Scholar
Jeschull, L. & Roeper, T. (2009). Evidentiality vs. certainty: Do children trust their minds more than their eyes? In Crawford, J. (ed.), Proceedings of the Third Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (pp. 107–115). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Koring, L. (2012). Don't shoot the messenger: how subjectivity affects distributional properties. Lingua 122, 874–90.Google Scholar
Koring, L. (2013). Seemingly similar: subjects and displacement in grammar, processing, and acquisition. Utrecht: LOT Publications.Google Scholar
Kratzer, A. (2010). Collected papers on modals and conditionals, chapter 2. Online: <semanticsarchive.net>..>Google Scholar
Matsui, T. & Fitneva, S. A. (2009). Knowing how we know: evidentiality and cognitive development. In Fitneva, S. A. & Matsui, T. (eds), Evidentiality: a window into language and cognitive development (New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 125) (pp. 111). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Matsui, T. & Miura, Y. (2009). Children's understanding of certainty and evidentiality: advantage of grammaticalized forms over lexical alternatives. In Fitneva, S. A. & Matsui, T. (eds), Evidentiality: a window into language and cognitive development (New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 125) (pp. 6377). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Matsui, T., Miura, Y. & McCagg, P. (2006b). Young children's implicit and explicit understanding of speaker knowledge. In Sun, R. (ed.), Proceedings of the 28th Annual Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1789–1794). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Matsui, T., Yamamoto, T. & McCagg, P. (2006a). On the role of language in children's early understanding of others as epistemic beings. Cognitive Development 12, 158–73.Google Scholar
Matushansky, O. (2002). Tipping the scales: the syntax of scalarity in the complement of seem . Syntax 5, 219–76.Google Scholar
Murray, S. (2010). Evidentiality and the structure of speech acts. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University.Google Scholar
Orfitelli, R. (2012). Argument intervention in the acquisition of A-movement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, UCLA.Google Scholar
Ozturk, O. & Papafragou, A. (2007). Children's acquisition of evidentiality. In Caunt-Nulton, H., Kulatilake, S. & Woo, I. (eds), Proceedings from the 31st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Online: <www.bu.edu/bucld/procedings/supplement/vol31>..' href=https://scholar.google.com/scholar?q=Ozturk,+O.+&+Papafragou,+A.+(2007).+Children's+acquisition+of+evidentiality.+In+Caunt-Nulton,+H.,+Kulatilake,+S.+&+Woo,+I.+(eds),+Proceedings+from+the+31st+Annual+Boston+University+Conference+on+Language+Development.+Online:+.>Google Scholar
Ozturk, O. & Papafragou, A. (2008). The acquisition of evidentiality and source monitoring. In Chan, H., Jacob, H. & Kapia, E. (eds), Proceedings from the 32nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 368–377). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A., Li, P., Choi, Y. & Han, C. (2007). Evidentiality in language and cognition. Cognition 103, 253–99.Google Scholar
Quené, H. & van den Bergh, H. (2008). Examples of mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects and with binomial data. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 413–25.Google Scholar
Rett, J. & Hyams, N. (2014). The acquisition of syntactically encoded evidentiality. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics 21, 173–98.Google Scholar
Robinson, E. J. (2009). Commentary: What can we learn from new research on evidentials? In Fitneva, S. A. & Matsui, T. (eds), Evidentiality: a window into language and cognitive development (New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 125) (pp. 95103). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
van Bruggen, E. (1980). Schijnen, lijken, blijken. TABU Taalkundig Bulletin 11, 5462.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J., Garfield, J., Gernet-Girard, H., Roeper, T. & Speas, M. (2009). Evidentials in Tibetan: acquisition, semantics and cognitive development. In Fitneva, S. A. & Matsui, T. (eds), Evidentiality: a window into language and cognitive development (New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 125) (pp. 2947). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Vliegen, M. (2011). Evidentiality. Dutch seem and appear verbs: blijken, lijken, schijnen. In Nouwen, R. & Elenbaas, M. (eds), Linguistics in the Netherlands (pp. 125137). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Koring Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Download Koring Supplementary Material(PDF)
PDF 175.4 KB