Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T12:07:09.588Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Task-appropriate input supports word–object association in 14-month-old female infants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2019

Laurel FAIS*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia
Eric VATIKIOTIS-BATESON
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics and, Cognitive Systems Program, University of British Columbia
*
*Corresponding author: 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, BCV6T 1Z4, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Fourteen-month-old infants are unable to link minimal pair nonsense words with novel objects (Stager & Werker, 1997). Might an adult's productions in a word learning context support minimal pair word–object association in these infants? We recorded a mother interacting with her 24-month-old son, and with her 5-month-old son, producing nonsense words bin and din. We used these productions to determine if they had a differential effect on 14-month-old infants’ word–object association abilities. Females hearing the words spoken to the older infant, but not those to the younger, succeeded. We suggest that the task-appropriateness of utterances can support infant word learning.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albert, R. R., Schwade, J. A., & Goldstein, M. H. (2017). The social functions of babbling: acoustic and contextual characteristics that facilitate maternal responsiveness. Developmental Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12641Google ScholarPubMed
Bauer, D., Goldfield, B., & Reznick, J. (2002). Alternative approaches to analyzing individual differences in the rate of early vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(3), 313–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P. (1997). Intentionality and word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1(1), 912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2005). Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Retrieved from <http://www.praat.org/>..>Google Scholar
Cohen, L. B., Atkinson, D. J., & Chaput, H. H. (2000). Habit 2000: a new program for obtaining and organizing data in infant perception and cognition studies. Version 1.0, 2000–2002.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intention in infancy. Mind & Language, 25(2), 141–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B., Hallé, P., Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1989). A crosslinguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling. Journal of Child Language, 16(1), 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fais, L., Werker, J. F., Cass, B., Leibowich, J., Vilela Barbosa, A., & Vatikiotis-Bateson, E. (2012). Here's looking at you, baby: what gaze and movement reveal about minimal pair word–object association at 14 months. Journal of Laboratory Phonology, 3, 91124.Google Scholar
Fennell, C. T., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). What paradox? Referential cues allow for infant use of phonetic detail in word learning. Child Development, 81(5), 1376–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2003). Early word learners’ ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech, 46(2), 245–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C.T., & Werker, J. F. (2004). Infant attention to phonetic detail: knowledge and familiarity effects. In Beachley, B., Brown, A., & Conlin, F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 165–76). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: user's guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Floccia, C., Keren-Portnoy, T., DePaolis, R., Duffy, H., Delle Luche, C., Durrant, S., White, L., Goslin, J., & Vihman, M. (2016). British English infants segment words only with exaggerated infant-directed speech stimuli. Cognition, 148, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogate, L. J., Walker-Andrews, A. S., & Bahrick, L. E. (2001). The intersensory origins of word comprehension: an ecological-dynamic systems view. Developmental Science, 4(1), 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language, 17(1), 171–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19(5), 515–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R. M., Can, D. D., Soderstrom, M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2015). (Baby)talk to me: the social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquisition. Early Directions in Psychological Science, 24(5), 339–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graf Estes, K., & Hurley, K. (2013). Infant-directed prosody helps infants map sounds to meanings. Infancy, 18(5), 797824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, K. M., Bernstein Ratner, N., & Newman, R. S. (2017). Infant-directed speech (IDS) vowel clarity and child language outcomes. Journal of Child Language, 44(5), 1140–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kampe, K. K., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). ‘Hey John’: signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with ‘mentalizing’, regardless of modality. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12), 5258–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D. (2003). Pitch and communicative intent in mother's speech: adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy, 4(1), 85110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd-Fox, S., Széplaki-Köllőd, B., Yin, J., & Csibra, G. (2015). Are you talking to me? Neural activations in 6-month-old infants in response to being addressed during natural interactions. Cortex, 70, 3548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, W., Golinkoff, R., Houston, D., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2011). Word learning in infant- and adult-directed speech. Language Learning and Development, 7(3), 185201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1995). Early lexical acquisition and the vocabulary spurt: a response to Goldfield & Reznick. Journal of Child Language, 22(2), 461–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, E., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children (pp. 109–49). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Niwano, K., & Sugai, K. (2002). Intonation contour of Japanese maternal infant-directed speech and infant vocal response. Japan Journal of Special Education, 39(6), 5968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paavola, L., Kunnari, S., Moilanen, I., & Lehtihalmes, M. (2005). The functions of maternal verbal responses to prelinguistic infants as predictors of early communicative and linguistic development. First Language, 25(2), 173–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parish-Morris, J., Hennon, E., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. Child Development, 78(4), 1265–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pater, J., Stager, C., & Werker, J. F. (2004). The perceptual acquisition of phonological contrasts. Language, 80(3), 384402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43, 549–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word learning tasks. Nature, 388(6640), 381–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., Barnett, R. K., & MacKain, K. (1983). The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes. Journal of Child Language, 10, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7(1), 5371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trainor, L. J., & Desjardins, R. N. (2002). Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 335–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F., Cohen, L. B., Lloyd, V., Stager, C. L., & Casasola, M. (1998). Acquisition of word–object associations by 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(6), 1289–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. L., & Hoyne, K. L. (1999). Infants’ learning about words and sounds in relation to objects. Child Development, 70(1), 6577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. L., Markman, E. M., & Fitzsimmons, C. M. (1994). Rapid word learning in 13- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 30(4), 553–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshida, K. A., Fennell, C. T., Swingley, D., & Werker, J. F. (2009). Fourteen-month-old infants learn similar-sounding words. Developmental Science, 12(3), 412–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zamuner, T. S., Fais, L., & Werker, J. F. (2014). Infants track word forms in early word–object associations. Developmental Science, 17(4), 481–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed