Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:07:50.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A story about a word: does narrative presentation promote learning of a spatial preposition in German two-year-olds?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2012

KERSTIN NACHTIGÄLLER*
Affiliation:
Bielefeld University, Germany
KATHARINA J. ROHLFING
Affiliation:
Bielefeld University, Germany
KARLA K. MCGREGOR
Affiliation:
Iowa University, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Kerstin Nachtigäller, Bielefeld University, CITEC-Emergentist Semantics Group, Universitätsstraße 21–23, Bielefeld 33615, Germany. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We trained forty German-speaking children aged 1;8–2;0 in their comprehension of UNTER [UNDER]. The target word was presented within semantically organized input in the form of a ‘narrative’ to the experimental group and within ‘unconnected speech’ to the control group. We tested children's learning by asking them to perform an UNDER-relation before, immediately after, and again one day after the training using familiarized and unfamiliarized materials. Compared to controls, the experimental group learned better and retained more. Children with advanced expressive lexicons in particular were aided in generalizing to unfamiliarized materials by the narrative presentation. This study extends our understanding of how narrations scaffold young children's enrichment of nascent word knowledge.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The research reported in this article was made possible by a Dilthey Fellowship (Research Initiative Focus on the Humanities) from the Volkswagen Foundation to Katharina Rohlfing. Many thanks to all children who participated in this study.

References

REFERENCES

Bauer, P. J. & Wewerka, S. S. (1995). One- to two-year-olds' recall of events: The more expressed, the more impressed. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 59, 475–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boland, A. M., Haden, C. A. & Ornstein, P. A. (2003). Boosting children's memory by training mothers in the use of an elaborative conversational style as an event unfolds. Journal of Cognition and Development 4, 3965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capone, N. & McGregor, K. (2005). The effect of semantic representation on toddlers' word retrieval. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48, 1468–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (2010). Beyond fast mapping. Language Learning and Development 6, 184205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, S. & Rohlfing, K. J. (2010). Discourse and lexical patterns in mothers' speech during spatial tasks: What role do spatial words play? In Iwasaki, S., Hoji, H., Clancy, P. M. & Sohn, S. O. (eds), Japanese/Korean Linguistics,Vol. 17, 117–33. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973). Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings. Cognition 3, 161–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, J., Beckman, M. E. & Munson, B. (2004). The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47, 421–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J. & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59 (5, Serial No. 242).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grieve, R., Hoogenraad, R. & Murray, D. (1977). On the young child's use of lexis and syntax in understanding locative instructions. Cognition 5, 235–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimm, H. & Doil, H. (2000). Elternfragebogen für die Früherkennung von Risikokindern (ELFRA-2). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Haden, C. A., Ornstein, P. A., Rudek, D. J. & Cameron, D. (2009). Reminiscing in the early years: Patterns of maternal elaborativeness and children's remembering. International Journal of Behavioral Development 33, 118–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: P. H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hayne, H. & Herbert, J. (2004). Verbal cues facilitate memory retrieval during infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 89, 127–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hills, T. T., Maouene, J., Riordan, B. & Smith, L. B. (2010). The associative structure of language: Contextual diversity in early word learning. Journal of Memory and Language 63, 259–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development 73, 418–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horst, J. S., Parsons, K. L. & Bryan, N. M. (2011). Get the story straight: Contextual repetition promotes word learning from storybooks. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horst, J. S. & Samuelson, L. K. (2008). Fast mapping but poor retention in 24-month-old infants. Infancy 13(2), 128–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, J. R. & Slobin, D. I. (1979). The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish. Journal of Child Language 6(3), 529–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labov, W. & Waletzky, J. (1973). Erzählanalyse: mündliche Versionen persönlicher Erfahrung. In Ihwe, J. (ed.), Literaturwissenschaft und Linguistik. Eine Auswahl Texte zur Theorie der Literaturwissenschaft, Band 2, 78126. Hamburg: Athenäum Fischer Taschenbuch Verlag.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (1984). Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology 9, 111–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, K. K. (2004). Developmental dependencies between lexical semantics and reading. In Addison Stone, C., Silliman, E. R., Ehren, B. J. & Apel, K. (eds), Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders, 302317. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McGregor, K. K., Rohlfing, K. J., Bean, A. & Marschner, E. (2009). Gesture as a support for word learning: The case of under. Journal of Child Language 36, 807828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meints, K., Plunkett, K., Harris, P. L. & Dimmock, D. (2002). What is ‘on’ and ‘under’ for 15-, 18- and 24- month-olds? Typicality effects in early comprehension of spatial prepositions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 20, 113–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nachtigäller, K. & Rohlfing, K. J. (2011). Mothers' talking about early object and action concepts during picture book reading. In Kümmerling-Meibauer, B. (ed.), Where literacy begins. Children's books from 0 to 3, 193208. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ornstein, P. A., Haden, C. A. & Hedrick, A. M. (2004). Learning to remember: Social-communicative exchanges and the development of children's memory skills. Developmental Review 24, 374–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Haden, C. A. & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother–child conversations about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development 8, 403430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Leyva, D., Sparks, A. & Grolnick, W. (2010). Maternal elaborative reminiscing increases low-income children's narrative skills relative to dialogic reading. Early Education and Development 21, 318–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Sparks, A. & Leyva, D. (2010). A review of parent interventions for preschool children's language and emergent literacy. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 10, 97117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, C. & Ehri, L. C. (1994). Reading storybooks to kindergartners helps them learn new vocabulary words. Journal of Educational Psychology 86, 5464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohlfing, K. J. (2001). No preposition required. The role of prepositions for the understanding of spatial relations in language acquisition. In Pütz, M., Niemeier, S. & Dirven, R. (eds), Applied cognitive linguistics I: Theory and language acquisition, 229–47. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Rohlfing, K. J. (2005). Learning prepositions. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education 12, 1317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohlfing, K. J. (2006). Facilitating the acquisition of UNDER by means of IN and ON – a training study in Polish. Journal of Child Language 33, 5169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohlfing, K. J. (2011). Exploring ‘associative talk’: When German mothers instruct their two year olds about spatial tasks. Dialogue and Discourse 2, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinha, C. (1983). Background knowledge, presupposition and canonicality. In Seiler, T. & Wannenmacher, W. (eds), Concept development and the development of word meaning, 269–96. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly 21, 360407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thom, E. E. & Sandhofer, C. M. (2009). More is more: The relationship between vocabulary size and word extension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 104(4), 466–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wilcox, S. & Palermo, D. S. (1974). ‘In’, ‘on’, and ‘under’ revisited. Cognition 3, 245–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar