Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T05:32:46.179Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prelinguistic vocalizations distinguish pointing acts*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 January 2015

THOMAS GRÜNLOH*
Affiliation:
University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Dresden, Germany, and Max-Planck-Institute for Psycholinguistics, the Netherlands
ULF LISZKOWSKI
Affiliation:
University of Hamburg, Germany, and Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen
*
[*]Address for correspondence: Thomas Grünloh, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus, Saxon Cochlear Implant Center (SCIC), Fetscherstraße 74, 01307 Dresden, Germany; tel: +49(0)351 458 7148; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The current study investigated whether point-accompanying characteristics, like vocalizations and hand shape, differentiate infants' underlying motives of prelinguistic pointing. We elicited imperative (requestive) and declarative (expressive and informative) pointing acts in experimentally controlled situations, and analyzed accompanying characteristics. Experiment 1 revealed that prosodic characteristics of point-accompanying vocalizations distinguished requestive from both expressive and informative pointing acts, with little differences between the latter two. In addition, requestive points were more often realized with the whole hand than the index finger, while this was the opposite for expressive and informative acts. Experiment 2 replicated Experiment 1, revealing distinct prosodic characteristics for requestive pointing also when the referent was distal and when it had an index-finger shape. Findings reveal that beyond the social context, point-accompanying vocalizations give clues to infants' underlying intentions when pointing.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (Eds.) (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21, 205–26.Google Scholar
Baumann, S. & Grice, M. (2006). The intonation of accessibility. Journal of Pragmatics 38, 1636–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2010). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (version 5.1·29) [Computer program], online: <http://www.praat.org/>..>Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language 2, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F. & Colonnesi, C. (2004). The role of declarative pointing in developing a theory of mind. Infancy 5, 291308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, T. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Infant Development 63, 1143.Google ScholarPubMed
Cochet, H. & Vauclair, J. (2010a). Features of spontaneous pointing gestures in toddlers. Gesture 10, 86107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochet, H. & Vauclair, J. (2010b). Pointing gestures produced by toddlers from 15 to 30 months: different functions, hand shapes and laterality patterns. Infant Behavior and Development 33, 432–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Odorico, L. (1984). Nonsegmental features in prelinguistic communications: an analysis of some types of infant cry and non-cry vocalizations. Journal of Infant Language 11, 1727.Google ScholarPubMed
D'Odorico, L. & Franco, F. (1991). Selective production of vocalization types in different communicative contexts. Journal of Infant Language 18, 475–99.Google Scholar
Dore, J., Franklin, M. B., Miller, R. T. & Ramer, R. H. (1976). Transitional phenomena in early language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 9, 1328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esteve-Gibert, N. & Prieto, P. (2012). Prosody signals the emergence of intentional communication in the first year of life: evidence from Catalan-babbling infants. Journal of Child Language 40, 126.Google ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. A., Peizer, D. B. & Weeks, T. A. (1973). Model-and-replica phonological grammar of an infant's first words. Lingua 3, 3565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flax, J., Lahey, M., Harris, K. & Boothroyd, A. (1991). Relations between prosodic variables and communication function. Journal of Child Language 18, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franco, F. & Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Infant Language 23, 307–36.Google ScholarPubMed
Furrow, D. (1984). Young children's use of prosody. Journal of Infant Language 11, 203–13.Google ScholarPubMed
Furrow, D., Podrouzek, W. & Moore, C. (1990). The acoustical analysis of children's use of prosody in assertive and directive contexts. First Language 10, 3749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galligan, R. (1987). Intonation with single words: purposive and grammatical use. Journal of Infant Language 14, 121.Google ScholarPubMed
Gros-Louis, J. & Wu, Z. (2012). Twelve-month-olds’ vocal production during pointing in naturalistic interactions: sensitivity to parents’ attention and responses. Infant Behavior and Development 35, 773–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean: explorations in the development of language. London: Edward Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoicka, E. & Gattis, M. (2012). Acoustic differences between humorous and sincere communicative intentions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 30, 531–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knudsen, B. & Liszkowski, U. (2012). Eighteen- and 24-month-old infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes. Developmental Science 15, 113–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebal, K., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2010). Infants’ use of shared experience in declarative pointing. Infancy 15, 545–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Albrecht, K., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2008). Infants’ visual and auditory communication when a partner is or is not visually attending. Infant Behavior and Development 31, 157–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T. & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science 7, 297307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T. & Tomasello, M. (2006). Twelve- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development 7, 173–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science 10, F17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Schäfer, M., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2009). Prelinguistic infants, but not chimpanzees, communicate about absent entities. Psychological Science 20, 654–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U. & Tomasello, M. (2011). Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing. Cognitive Development 26, 1629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcos, H. (1987). Communicative functions of pitch range and pitch direction in infants. Journal of Infant Language 14, 255–86.Google ScholarPubMed
McCune, L.Vihman, M. M., Roug-Hellichius, L., Delery, D. B. & Gogate, L. (1996). Grunt communication in human infants (Homo Sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology 110, 2737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oller, D. K., Buder, E. H., Ramsdell, H. L., Warlaumont, A. S., Chorna, L. & Bakeman, R. (2013). Functional flexibility of infant vocalization and the emergence of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110, 6318–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierrehumbert, J. B. & Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In Cohen, P. R., Morgan, J. & Pollack, M. E. (eds), Intentions in communication, 271311. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochat, P. & Goubet, N. (1995). Development of sitting and reaching in 5- to 6-month-old infants. Infant Behavior & Development 18, 5368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, D. (2004). Falling intonation in the one- and two-syllable utterances of infants and preschoolers. Journal of Phonetics 32, 373–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, D. & Balog, H. L. (2002). Do infants produce the melody before the words? A review of developmental intonation research. Lingua 112, 1025–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M. & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Infant Development 78, 705–22.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. & Miller, R. (1988). Words and babble at the threshold of language acquisition. In Smith, M. D. & Locke, J. L. (eds), The emergent lexicon: the infant's development of a linguistic vocabulary, 151–84. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. & Wharton, T. (2006). Relevance and prosody. Journal of Pragmatics 38, 1559–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar