Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T17:09:45.712Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pointing and social awareness: declaring and requesting in the second year*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Fabia Franco*
Affiliation:
University of Padova
George Butterworth
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
*
Address for correspondence: Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della Socializzazione, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The production of pointing and other gestures (e.g. reaching or indicative gestures) by 47 infants aged 1; 0 to 1; 6 was investigated in two experiments contrasting declarative-referential vs. imperative-instrumental conditions of communication. A further group of seven infants aged 0; 10 was examined in order to highlight pre-pointing transitional phenomena. Data analyses concerned gestures and associated vocalizations and visual checking with a social partner. Results show that gestures are produced differentially in the experimental conditions: while reaching is only produced in imperative-instrumental contexts, pointing is characteristic of declarative-referential contexts. The pattern of visual checking with the social partner also differentiates gestures; moreover, it shows developmental changes in the case of pointing. Results suggest that pointing relies on some awareness of ‘psychological’ processes (e.g. attention and sharing) in the other and the self, and that it is this which may account for the specific relevance of pointing for language development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The studies reported here were conducted when both authors were at the University of Stirling (Scotland); pilot work was funded by Italian C.N.R. 08 A.I. grant and a European Science Foundation short term fellowship, whereas the main project was funded by ESRC grant N. R000231286. Preliminary parts of this research have been presented at the 1989 and 1991 SRCD conferences. Many thanks are due to the technical staff of Stirling Department of Psychology for co-operation during various phases of the studies; to Adina Lew for helping with reliability; and to Eraldo Nicotra for discussions about statistical issues.

References

REFERENCES

Adamson, L. B. & Bakeman, R. (1982). Affectivity and reference: concepts, methods and techniques in the study of communication development of six to eighteen month old infants. In Field, T. M. & Fogel, A. (eds), Emotion and interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R. & Smith, C. B. (1990). Gestures, words, and early object sharing. In Volterra, V. & Erling, C. J. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development 62, 875–90.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role-taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 7, 113–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merril Palmer Quarterly 21, 205–26.Google Scholar
Bates, E., O'Connell, B. & Shore, C. (1987). Language and communication in infancy. In Osofsky, J. D. (ed.), Handbook of Infant Development. Second edition. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language 2, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child's talk; learning to use the language. Oxford: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. & Franco, F. (1993). Motor development: communication and cognition. In Kalverboer, L., Hopkins, B. & Gueze, R. H. (eds), A longitudinal approach to the study of motor development in early and later childhood. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. & Grover, L. (1988). The origins of referential communication in human infancy. In Weiskrantz, L. (ed.), Thought without language. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Camaioni, L. (1992). Mind knowledge in infancy: The emergence of intentional communication. Early development and parenting, I, 1522.Google Scholar
Dobrich, W. & Scarborough, H. S. (1984). Form and function in early communication: language and pointing gestures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 475–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Odorico, L. & Levorato, C. (1990). Social and cognitive determinants of mutual gaze between mother and infant. In Volterra, V. & Erling, C. J. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1983). Feeling, form, and intention in the baby's transition to language. In Golinkoff, R. Michnick (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fogel, A. & Hannan, T. E. (1985). Manual actions of 2- to 3-month-old human infants during social interaction. Child Development 56, 1271–79.Google Scholar
Fogel, A. & Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and communicative action; reinterpreting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Developmental Psychology 23, 747–61.Google Scholar
Goldfield, B. A. (1988). Pointing, naming, and talk about toys: referential behavior in children and mothers. Paper presented at the 6th Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Washington D.C.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1993). When is communication a ‘meeting of minds’? Journal of Child Language 20, 199207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, J. C. (1991). Visual behaviour as a window for reading the mind of others in primates. In Whiten, A. (ed.), Natural theories of mind. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean. London: Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harding, C. G. & Golinkoff, R. M. (1979). The origins of intentional vocalizations in prelingustic infants. Child Development 50, 3340.Google Scholar
Kessler-Shaw, L. (1992). Maternal object and action references in response to infant gestures and other attention-indicating actions. The City University of New York, Graduate School report.Google Scholar
Klinnert, M. D. (1984). The regulation of infant behavior by maternal facial expression. Infant Behavior and Development 7, 447–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leung, E. & Rheingold, H. (1981). The development of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology 17, 215–20.Google Scholar
Lock, A., Young, A., Service, V. & Chandler, P. (1990). Some observations on the origin of the pointing gesture. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Marcos, H. (1991). How do adults contribute to the development of early referential communication? European Journal of Psychology of Education 3, 271–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1990). Gestural development, dual-directional signaling, and the transition to words. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1987). Psycholinguistics. A new approach. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J. & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficit of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 27, 657–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, C. (1978). Pointing in the context of a shared activity. Child Development 49, 371–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, C. & Messer, D. (1977). Mothers, infants and pointing: a study of gesture. In Schaffer, R. H. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pechmann, T. & Deutsch, W. (1982). The development of verbal and nonverbal devices for reference. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 34, 330–41.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Nelson, K. E. & Boysen, S. T. (1992). Comprehension of role reversal in chimpanzees: evidence of empathy? Animal Behavior 43, 633–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S. E., Murphy, J., Sevick, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L. & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 58, Nos. 3–4 (Serial No. 233).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaffer, H. R. (1984). The child's entry into a social world. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). The social bases of language acquisition. Social Development 1, 6787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1926, 1962). Thought and language. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963, 1984). Symbol formation. Reprinted Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (ed.) (1991). Natural theories of mind. Oxford; Blackwell.Google Scholar
Winer, B. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar