Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:25:53.096Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The perfective past tense in Greek child language*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 September 2008

STAVROULA STAVRAKAKI
Affiliation:
University of Thessaloniki
HARALD CLAHSEN*
Affiliation:
University of Essex
*
Address for correspondence: Harald Clahsen, Department of Linguistics, University of Essex, Colchester, C04 3SQ, UK. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study examines the perfective past tense of Greek in an elicited production and an acceptability judgment task testing 35 adult native speakers and 154 children in six age groups (age range: 3 ; 5 to 8 ; 5) on both existing and novel verb stimuli. We found a striking contrast between sigmatic and non-sigmatic perfective past tense forms. Sigmatic forms (which have a segmentable perfective affix (-s-) in Greek) were widely generalized to different kinds of novel verbs in both children and adults and were overgeneralized to existing non-sigmatic verbs in children's productions. By contrast, non-sigmatic forms were only extended to novel verbs that were similar to existing non-sigmatic verbs, and overapplications of non-sigmatic forms to existing sigmatic verbs were extremely rare. We argue that these findings are consistent with dual-mechanism accounts of morphology.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We thank Gaberell Drachman, Claudia Felser, Despoina Papadopoulou, Ursula Stephany, Evangelia Thomadaki and two anonymous JCL reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper. We are also grateful to Anna Anastasiadi for discussions on Greek verb morphology, Giorgos Orfanos for letting us have access to the Neurosoft corpus and João Verissimo for statistical advice. Thanks also go to Nikoletta Dalatsi and Eleni Vletsi for helping us count the verb lemmas in the Neurosoft corpus, and to the undergraduate students of the Department of Speech and Language Therapy at the Technological Educational Institute of Epirus in Ioannina who collected the data for the present study under the supervision of the first author.

References

REFERENCES

Brown, R. & Bellugi, U. (1964). Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational Review 34, 133–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (1991). Natural morphology: The organization of paradigms and language acquisition. In Huebner, T. & Ferguson, C. (eds) Crosscurrents in second language acquisition and linguistic theories, 6792. Benjamins: Amsterdam.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995) Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language & Cognitive Processes 10, 425–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H. (2006). Dual-mechanism morphology. In: Brown, K. (ed.) Encyclopedia of language and linguistics, 2nd ed., Vol. 4, 15. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Aveledo, F. & Roca, I. (2002). The development of regular and irregular verb inflection in Spanish child language. Journal of Child Language 29, 591622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clahsen, H. & Rothweiler, M. (1993). Inflectional rules in children's grammars: Evidence from the development of participles in German. Yearbook of Morphology 1992, 134.Google Scholar
Dabrowska, E. & Szczerbinski, M. (2006). Polish children's productivity with case marking: The role of regularity type frequency and phonological diversity. Journal of Child Language 33, 559–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delidaki, S. & Varlokosta, S. (2003). Testing the aspect first hypothesis: A preliminary investigation into the comprehension of tense in child Greek. ZAS Papers in Linguistics 29, 7384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, H. T. & Kirkpatrick, A. G. (1999). Metalinguistic awareness in children: A developmental progression. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 28, 313–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J., Bates, L., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics 6, 6570.Google Scholar
Holton, D., Mackridge, P. & Phillipaki-Warburton, I. (1997). Greek: A comprehensive grammar of the modern language. London: Routlege.Google Scholar
Katis, D. (1984). Acquisition of the Modern Greek Imperfective Past. Studies in Greek Linguistics: Proceedings of the 4th Annual Meeting of the Department of Linguistics, 185217. Thessaloniki: University of Thessaloniki.Google Scholar
Kehayia, E. & Jarema, G. (1991). On agrammatism in Greek. Neurolinguistik 35, 4761.Google Scholar
Lahha, S., Ravid, D., Korecky-Kröll, K., Laaha, G. & Dressler, W. U. (2006). Early noun plurals in German: Regularity, productivity or default? Journal of Child Language 33, 271302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. (2000). Grammar and conceptualization. Berlin, New York: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Plunkett, K. & Goodman, J. (1997). Overregularization in English plural and past tense inflectional morphology: A response to Marcus (1995). Journal of Child Language 24, 767–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M. T., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J. & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 57 (Serial No. 228). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mastropavlou, M. (2007). The role of phonological saliency in the acquisition of past tense morphology by Greek children with Specific Language Impairment. In Agathopoulou, E., Dimitrakopoulou, M. & Papadopoulou, D. (eds) Selected Papers on Theoretical and Applied Linguistics from the 17th International Symposium, 453–63. Thessaloniki: University of Thessaloniki.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. & Patterson, K. (2002). Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 465–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeill, D. (1966). Developmental psycholinguistics. In: Smith, F. & Miller, G. (eds) The genesis of language: A psycholinguistic approach, 1884. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Orsolini, M., Fanari, R. & Bowles, H. (1998). Acquiring regular and irregular inflection in a language with verb classes. Language & Cognitive Processes 13, 425–64.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. & Ullman, M. (2002). The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 456–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ragnarsdottir, H., Simonsen, H. G. & Plunkett, K. (1999). The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: An experimental study. Journal of Child Language 26, 577618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ralli, A. (1988). Eléments de la morphologie du grec moderne: la structure du verbe. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Montreal.Google Scholar
Ralli, A. (2003). Morphology in Greek Linguistics: The state of the art. Journal of Greek Linguistics 4, 77129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royle, P. (2007). Variable effects of morphology and frequency on inflection patterns in French preschoolers. The Mental Lexicon 2, 103125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Say, T. & Clahsen, H. (2002). Words, rules and stems in the Italian mental lexicon. In Nooteboom, S., Weerman, F. & Wijnen, F. (eds) Storage and computation in the language faculty, 93129. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaffer, J. Popper (1986). Modified sequentially rejective multiple test procedures. Journal of the American Statistical Association 81, 826–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephany, U. (1997). The acquisition of Greek. In Slobin, D. (ed.) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 4, 183303. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Press.Google Scholar
Stephany, U. & Voeikova, M. (2003). On the early development of aspect in Greek and Russian child language: A comparative analysis. ZAS Papers in Linguistics 29, 211–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terzi, A., Papapetropoulos, S. & Kouvelas, E. (2005). Past tense formation and comprehension of passive sentences in Parkinson's disease: Evidence from Greek. Brain & Language 94, 297303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Triandafillidis, M. (1941). Neolliniki grammatiki [Modern Greek grammar]. Athens: OESV.Google Scholar
Tsapkini, K., Jarema, G. & Kehayia, E. (2001). Manifestations of morphological impairments in Greek aphasia: A case study. Journal of Neurolinguistics 14, 281–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsapkini, K., Jarema, G. & Kehayia, E. (2002 a). A morphological processing deficit in verbs but not in nouns: A case study in a highly inflected language. Journal of Neurolinguistics 15, 265–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsapkini, K., Jarema, G. & Kehayia, E. (2002 b). The role of verbal morphology in aphasia during lexical access: Evidence from Greek. In Fava, E. (eds) Clinical linguistics and phonetics: Theory and applications in speech pathology and therapy, 299314. Amsterdam/Philadephia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar