Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:16:57.177Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On-line sentence processing in Swedish: cross-linguistic developmental comparisons with French*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2011

MICHÈLE KAIL*
Affiliation:
Laboratoire Structures Formelles du Langage, CNRS, UMR 7023 and Université de Paris VIII Saint Denis
MARIA KIHLSTEDT
Affiliation:
CNRS UMR 7114 MoDyCo and Université de Paris Ouest Nanterre La Défense
PHILIPPE BONNET
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie et Neuropsychologie Cognitive FRE 3292, CNRS-Paris Descartes
*
Address for correspondence: Michèle Kail, Laboratoire Structures Formelles du Langage, Centre CNRS Pouchet, 59 rue Pouchet, 75849 Paris cedex 17, France. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study examined on-line processing of Swedish sentences in a grammaticality-judgement experiment within the framework of the Competition Model. Three age groups from 6 to 11 and an adult group were asked to detect grammatical violations as quickly as possible. Three factors concerning cue cost were studied: violation position (early vs. late), violation span (intraphrasal vs. interphrasal) and violation type (agreement vs. word order). Developmental results showed that children were always slower at detecting grammatical violations. Irrespective of age, participants were faster at judging sentences with late violations, especially in the younger groups. Intraphrasal violations were more rapidly detected than interphrasal ones, particularly in adults. Finally, agreement violations and word order ones did not differ. The hierarchy of cue cost factors indicated that violation span was the dominant one. A cross-linguistic analysis with French (Kail, 2004) underlines the developmental processing abilities and the interdependence between cue cost and cue validity.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by a ‘School and Cognitive Sciences’ fellowship from the French Ministry of Research.

References

REFERENCES

Allen, S. (1971). Nusvensk frekvensordbok 2: Lemman. Stockholm:Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M. (1989). Parsing and interpretation: An introduction. Language and Cognition Processes 4, 119.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Devescovi, A. & Wulfeck, B. (2001). Psycholinguistics: A cross-language perspective. Annual Review of Psychology 52, 369–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, A., Bates, E. & Fisher, D. (1996). The time course of grammaticality judgement. Language and Cognitive Processes 11, 337406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charvillat, A. & Kail, M. (1991). The status of canonical SVO sentences in French: A developmental study of the on-line processing of dislocated sentences. Journal of Child Language 18, 591608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H. & Muysken, P. (1989). The UG paradox in L2 Acquisition. Second Language Research 5, 129.Google Scholar
Cohen, J., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M. & Provost, J. (1993). PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers 25(2), 257–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, L. (2004). The acquisition of tense. In Josefsson, G., Platzack, C. & Håkansson, G. (eds), The Acquisition of Swedish Grammar, 3174. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuetos, F. & Mitchell, D. C. (1988). Cross-linguistic differences in parsing: Restrictions on the use of the late closure strategy in Spanish. Cognition 30, 73105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devescovi, A. & D'Amico, S. (2005). The competition model: Cross-linguistic studies of on-line processing. In Tomasello, M. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Beyond nature–nurture. Essays in honor of Elizabeth Bates, 165–91. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Devescovi, A., D'Amico, S. & Gentile, P. (1999). The development of sentence comprehension in Italian: A reaction time study. First Language 19, 129–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabrizio, A., Guasti, M. T. & Adani, F. (2006). Relative clause processing by Italian children: A self-paced listening study. Paper presented at the Workshop on On-line Methods in Children's Language Processing, Graduate Center of the City University of New York (CUNY).Google Scholar
Frazier, L. (1987). Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In Coltheart, M. (ed.). Attention and performance II, 559–86. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Friederici, A., Weissenborn, J. & Kail, M. (1991). Pronoun comprehension in aphasia: A comparison of three languages. Brain and Language 41, 289310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullberg, M. (1994). Who is doing what to whom? Testing the Competition Model on Swedish. Working Papers at the Department of Linguistics and Phonetics, Lund University.Google Scholar
Håkansson, G. (2005). Similiarities and differences in L1 and L2 development. Opening up the perspective: Including SLI. In Pienemann, M. (ed.), Cross-linguistic aspects of processability theory, 179–97. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jörgensen, N. (1976). Meningsbyggnaden i talad svenska. Funktion och byggnad. Lundastudier i nordisk språkvetenskap, Lund: Studentlitteratur.Google Scholar
Kail, M. (1989). Cue validity, cue cost, and processing types in sentence comprehension in French and Spanish. In MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (eds), The cross-linguistic study of sentence processing, 77117. Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kail, M. (1999). Linguistic variations and cognitive constraints in the processing and the acquisition of language. In Fuchs, C. & Robert, S. (eds), Language diversity and cognitive representations, 179–94. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, M. (2004). On-line grammaticality judgments in French children and adults: A cross-linguistic perspective. Journal of Child Language 31, 713–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, M. & Bassano, D. (1997). Verb agreement processing in French: A study of on-line grammaticality judgments. Language and Speech 40(1), 2546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, M., & Charvillat, A. (1988). Local and topological processing in sentence comprehension by French and Spanish children. Journal of Child Language 15, 637–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, M.Costa, A. & Hub Faria, I. (2008). Jugements de grammaticalité en temps réel: étude comparative du français et du portugais. In Kail, M., Fayol, M. & Hickmann, M. (eds), L'apprentissage des langues, 199222. Paris: CNRS Editions.Google Scholar
Kail, M. & Diakogiorgi, K. (1998). On-line integration of morpho-syntactic cues by Greek children and adults: A cross-linguistic perspective. In Dittmar, N. & Penner, Z. (eds), Issues in the theory of language acquisition, 177201. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Kempe, V. & MacWhinney, B. (1999). Processing of morphological and semantic cues in Russian and German. Language and Cognitive Processes 14(2), 129–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindner, K. (2003). The development of sentence interpretation strategies in monolingual German learning children with and without specific language impairment. Linguistics 41, 213–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1987). The competition model. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition, 73136. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (eds), (1989). The cross-linguistic study of sentence processing. Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., St John, M. & Taraban, R. (1989). Sentence comprehension: A parallel distributed processing approach. Language and Cognitive Processes 4, 287335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. (1986). The development of sentence comprehension strategies in English and Dutch. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 41, 317–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, D. C. (1994). Sentence parsing. In Gernsbacher, M. L (ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics, 375409. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Moscoso del Prado, Martin F., Kostic, A. & Baayen, R. H. (2004). Putting the bits together: An information theoretical perspective on morphological processing. Cognition 94, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L., Marinis, T., Felser, C. & Clahsen, H. (2007). Antecedent priming at trace positions in children's sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 36(2), 175–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sekerina, I. A., Fernandez, E. M. & Clahsen, H. (eds), (2008), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children's language processing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (ed.) (1985). The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Staron, M., Bokus, B. & Kail, M. (2005). On-line sentence processing in Polish children and adults. In Bokus, B. (ed.), Studies in the psychology of language, 227–45. Warsaw: Matrix.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M., Carlsson, G. & Trueswell, J. C. (1989). The role of thematic structure in interpretation and parsing. Language and Cognitive Processes 4, 211–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, G. R., Lambert, W. E. & Rigault, A. A. (1977). The French speaker's skill with grammatical gender. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, L. K. & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1981). Children's processing of spoken language. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20, 400416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J. C. (2008). Using eye movements as a developmental measure within psycholinguistics. In Sekerina, I. A., Fernandez, E. M & Clahsen, H. (eds), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children's language processing, 7396. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissenborn, J., Kail, M. & Friederici, A. (1990). Language particular or language independent factors in acquisition. First Language 10, 141–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wulfeck, B. (1993). A reaction time study of grammaticality judgments in children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36(6), 1208–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed