Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:53:01.372Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Noun-pronoun pleonasms: the role of age and situation*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Eleanor G. Cotton
Affiliation:
University of Texas at El Paso

Abstract

The process of topicalization occasionally produces pleonasms with nominal-pronominal reduplication (NPR). In these structures the topic nominal is often accompanied by a comment clause containing a pronoun which is co-referential with the topic. This article discusses NPR in the language of children ages seven and nine in four different situations, one of which was child-child in free play, three of which were child-adult in retelling stories, describing pictures, and answering questions. The younger children produced more NPR. Influenced by their attitudes toward their interlocutors, the content of the situations and themselves as communicators, all of the children produced little NPR talking to their peers and increasing amounts talking to adults. Examples are given and analysed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1978

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Brown, R. & Bellugi-Klima, U. (1971). Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. In Bar-Adon, A. & Leopold, W. (eds), Child language: a book of readings. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Chambers, J. (1973). Remarks on topicalization in child language. FL 9. 442–6.Google Scholar
Chomsky, C. (1969). The acquisition of syntax in children from 5 to 10. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Cotton, E. (1973). Child language: topicalization and nominal-pronominal reduplication as a function of language universals, language development, and language situation. Doctoral dissertation, University of New Mexico.Google Scholar
Fasold, R. & Wolfram, W. (1972). Some linguistic features of negro dialect. In Contemporary English: change and variation. Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Firth, J. R. (1964). On sociological linguistics. In Hymes, D. (ed.), Language in culture and society. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Gruber, J. (1967). Topicalization in child language. FL 3. 3765.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. (1975). Learning how to mean. In , E. H. & Lenneberg, E. (eds), Foundations of language development: a multidisciplinary approach. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. (1968). The problem of universals. In Greenberg, J. (ed.), Universals of language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Jesperson, O. (1933). Language. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Menyuk, P. (1969). Sentences children use. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Quirk, R. & Greenbaum, S. (1973). A concise grammar of contemporary English. New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.Google Scholar