Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T17:11:23.087Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 November 2009

PAOLA PETTENATI*
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università di Parma, Italy
SILVIA STEFANINI
Affiliation:
Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università di Parma, Italy
VIRGINIA VOLTERRA
Affiliation:
Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, Italy
*
Address for correspondence: Paola Pettenati, Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università di Parma, via Volturno 39/E 43100 Parma. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study explores the form of representational gestures produced by forty-five hearing children (age range 2 ; 0–3 ; 1) asked to label pictures in words. Five pictures depicting objects and five pictures depicting actions which elicited more representational gestures were chosen for more detailed analysis. The range of gestures produced for each item varied from 3 to 27 for a total of 128 gestures. Gestures have been analyzed with the same parameters used to describe signs produced by deaf children: handshape, location and movement. Results show that gestures for a given picture exhibit similarities in many of the parameters across children. Some motor characteristics found in the production of hearing toddlers' gestures are similar to those described for early signs. Implications of this similarity between gestural and signed linguistic representations in young children are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The work reported in this paper was supported by the Fondazione Monte di Parma (Research Group for Study on Children's Motor and Language Development, University of Parma). We are very grateful to the participants of the Nascent Languages Conference, Rockefeller Foundation (Bellagio, Italy, 4–8 October 2006) for their insightful suggestions on an early presentation of the study. We thank Elena Congestrì for her collaboration in data transcription, Penny Boyes-Braem, Tommaso Russo and Bencie Woll for their helpful comments on previous versions of this manuscript, and Aldo Di Domenico for figures drawing. We especially thank the children who participated in the study, their parents and their schools. Author contributions: VV conceived The idea; SS designed the setting and collected the data; PP analyzed the data; PP, SS VV wrote the paper.

References

REFERENCES

Acredolo, L. P. & Goodwin, S. W. (1994). Sign language among hearing infants: The spontaneous development of symbolic gestures. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children, 6878. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Kita, S. & Young, A. J. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes 15, 593613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ann, J. (1996). On the relation between ease of articulation and frequency of occurrence of handshapes in two sign languages. Lingua 98, 1941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Oztop, E. & Zukow-Goldring, P. (2005). Language and the mirror system: a perception/action based approach to communicative development. Cogniţie, Creier, Comportament/Cognition, Brain, Behavior IX(3), 239–72.Google Scholar
Boyes-Braem, P. (1994). Acquisition of handshape in American Sign Language: a preliminary analysis. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children, 107127. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Butcher, C. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one to two word speech: when hand and mouth come together. In McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and gesture, 235–58. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V. (2005). From action to language through gesture: a longitudinal perspective. Gesture 5, 155–77.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C. & Casadio, P. (1995). Il primo vocabolario del bambino. Guida all'uso del questionario MacArthur per la valutazione della comunicazione e del linguaggio nei primi anni di vita. Milano: Franco Angeli.Google Scholar
Cheek, A., Cormier, K., Repp, A. & Meier, R. P. (2001). Prelinguistic gesture predicts mastery and error in the production of first signs. Language 77, 292323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conlin, K. E., Mirus, G. R., Mauk, C. & Meier, R. P. (2000). The acquisition of first signs: place, handshape and movement. In Chamberlain, C., Morford, J. P. & Mayberry, R. I. (eds), Language acquisition by eye, 5169. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fagard, J. (1994). Manual strategies and interlimb coordination during reaching, grasping, and manipulating throughout the first year of life. In Swinnen, S. P., Heuer, H., Massion, J. & Casaer, P. (eds), Interlimb coordination: Neural, dynamical, and cognitive constraints, 461–90. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gallese, V. (2000). The inner sense of action: agency and motor representations. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7, 2340.Google Scholar
Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P. & Brown, C. A. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24, 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9, 2343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Volterra, V. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: Early communication of Italian vs. American children. First Language 28(2), 164–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth and brain. The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6, 1940.Google Scholar
Juncos, O., Caamano, A., Justo, M. J., Lopez, E., Rivas, R. M., Lopez, Y. et al. (1997). Primeras palabras en la Legua de Signos Espanola (LSE). Estructura formal, semantica y contextual. Revisita de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología 17, 170–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karnopp, L. B. (2002). Phonology acquisition in Brazilian Sign Language. In Morgan, G. and Woll, B. (eds), Directions in sign language acquisition, 2953. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004) Gesture: visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, J., Atkinson, J., Smulovitch, E., Tracker, A. & Woll, B. (2004). Aphasia in a user of British Sign Language: dissociation between sign and gesture. Cognitive Neuropsychology 21(5), 537–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, R. & Squires, B. (2006). Sign language acquisition. In Lieven, E. (ed.), Language acquisition. Encyclopaedia of language and linguisitcs, Vol. 11, 2nd edn, 291–95. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
McIntire, M. (1977). The acquisition of American Sign Language hand configurations. Sign Language Studies 16, 247–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, R. P. (2006). The form of early signs: explaining signing children's articulatory development. In Schick, B., Marschark, M. & Spencer, P. E. (eds.), Advances in the sign language development of deaf children, 202230. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meier, R. P., Mauk, C., Cheek, A. & Moreland, C. (2008). The form of children's early signs: Iconic or motor determinants? Language Learning and Development 4, 6398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, R. P. & Willerman, R. (1995). Prelinguistic gesture in deaf and hearing babies. In Emmorey, K. & Reilly, J. S. (eds), Language, gesture and space, 391409. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Morford, J. P., Singleton, J. L. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1995). The genesis of language: how much time is needed to generate arbitrary symbols in a sign system? In Emorrey, K. & Reilly, J. (eds), Language, gesture and space, 313–32. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Morgan, G., Barrett-Jones, S. & Stoneham, H. (2007). The first signs of language: Phonological development in British Sign Language. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pietrandrea, P. (1997). I dizionari della LIS: analisi qualitative e quantitative. In Caselli, M. C. & Corazza, S. (eds), LIS. Studi, esperienze e ricerche sulla lingua dei segni in Italia, 255–59. Pisa: Edizioni del Cerro.Google Scholar
Pine, K. J., Lufkin, N. & Messer, D. (2004). More gesture than answers: children learning about balance. Developmental Psychology 40(6), 1059–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pizzuto, E. & Capobianco, M. (2005). The link and differences between deixis and symbols in children's early gestural-vocal system. Gesture 5, 179–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schembri, A., Jones, C. & Burnham, D. (2005). Comparing action gestures and classifier verbs of motion: evidence from Australian Sign Language, Taiwan Sign Language and non-signers gestures without speech. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10(3), 272–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singleton, J. L., Goldin-Meadow, S. & McNeill, D. (1995). The cataclysm break between gesticulation and sign: evidence against a unified continuum of gestural communication. In Emorrey, C. J. & Reilly, J. (eds), Language, gesture and space, 287311. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Stefanini, S., Bello, A., Caselli, M. C., Iverson, J. & Volterra, V. (2009). Spoken and gestural lexicon in a naming task: developmental data. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(2), 168–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton-Spence, R. L. & Woll, B. (1999). The linguistics of British Sign Language: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (1994). From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Iverson, I. M. & Castrataro, M. (2006). The development of gesture in hearing and deaf children. In Shick, B., Marschark, M. & Spencer, P. (eds), Advances in the sign language development of deaf children, 4670. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Wiesendanger, M., Wicki, U. & Rouiller, E. M. (1994). Are there unifying structures in the brain responsible for interlimb coordination? In Swinnen, S. P., Heuer, H., Massion, J. & Casaer, P. (eds), Interlimb coordination: neural, dynamical and cognitive constraints, 179207. San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar