Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T05:37:59.555Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Monodialectal and multidialectal infants’ representation of familiar words*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 March 2014

SAMANTHA DURRANT*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Plymouth University, UK
CLAIRE DELLE LUCHE
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Plymouth University, UK
ALLEGRA CATTANI
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Plymouth University, UK
CAROLINE FLOCCIA
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Plymouth University, UK
*
Address for correspondence: Samantha Durrant, School of Psychology, Plymouth University, Drake Circus, PL4 8AA, Plymouth, UK. tel: +44 1752584859; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Monolingual infants are typically studied as a homogenous group and compared to bilingual infants. This study looks further into two subgroups of monolingual infants, monodialectal and multidialectal, to identify the effects of dialect-related variation on the phonological representation of words. Using an Intermodal Preferential Looking task, the detection of mispronunciations in familiar words was compared in infants aged 1;8 exposed to consistent (monodialectal) or variable (multidialectal) pronunciations of words in their daily input. Only monodialectal infants detected the mispronunciations whereas multidialectal infants looked longer at the target following naming whether the label was correctly produced or not. This suggests that variable phonological input in the form of dialect variation impacts the degree of specificity of lexical representations in early infancy.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by grants from the Nuffield Foundation (2008–2011) and the ESRC (2009–2011) awarded to the last author. Special thanks are also due to all the toddlers and their parents who participated in this study.

References

REFERENCES

Albareda-Castellot, B., Pons, F. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2011). The acquisition of phonetic categories in bilingual infants: new data from an anticipatory eye movement paradigm. Developmental Science 14(2), 395401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altendorf, U. & Watt, D. (2004). The dialects in the South of England: phonology. Handbook of Varieties of English 1, 178203.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., Tyler, M. D., Gooding, T. N., Orlando, C. B. & Quann, C. A. (2009). Development of phonological constancy: toddlers' perception of native- and Jamaican-accented words. Psychological Science 20(5), 539–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F. & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13(4), 525–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life. Language and Speech 46(2/3), 217–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattani, A., Abbot-Smith, K., Farag, R., Krott, A., Arreckx, F., Dennis, I. & Floccia, C. (in press). How much exposure to English is necessary for a bilingual toddler to perform like a monolingual peer in language tests? International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders.Google Scholar
Fennell, C. T., Byers-Heinlein, K. & Werker, J. F. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: the case of bilingual infants. Child Development 78(5), 1510–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floccia, C., Delle Luche, C., Durrant, S., Butler, J. & Goslin, J. (2012). Parent or community: Where do 20-month-olds exposed to two accents acquire their representation of words? Cognition 124, 95100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, A., Plunkett, K. & Schafer, G. (2000). Infant vocabulary development assessed with a British Communicative Development Inventory. Journal of Child Language 27(3), 689705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, A. & Trudgill, P. (1996). English accents and dialects: an introduction to social and regional varieties of English in the British Isles (3rd ed.). London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2007). Fourteen-month-olds pay attention to vowels in novel words. Developmental Science 11(1), 53–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2010). Twelve-month-olds know their cups from their keps and tups. Infancy 15(5), 445–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meints, K. & Woodford, A. (2008). Lincoln Infant Lab Package 2008: a new programme package for IPL, preferential listening, habituation and eyetracking [www document: Computer software & manual]. Online: <http://www.lincoln.ac.uk/psychology/babylab.htm>..>Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., Lewedeg, V. & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics 18, 4158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C. & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning 43(1), 93120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Bialystok, E., Blaye, A., Polonia, A. & Yott, J. (2013). Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 17(1), 5770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramon-Casas, M. & Bosch, L. (2010). Are non-cognate words phonologically better specified than cognates in the early lexicon of bilingual children? In Ortega-Llebaria, M. (ed.), Selected proceedings of the 4th Conference on Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonology, 31–6. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Ramon-Casas, M., Swingley, D., Sebastián-Gallés, N. & Bosch, L. (2009). Vowel categorization during word recognition in bilingual toddlers. Cognitive Psychology 59(1), 96121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rost, G. C. & McMurray, B. (2009). Speaker variability augments phonological processing in early word learning. Developmental Science 12(2), 339–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rost, G. C. & McMurray, B. (2010). Finding the signal by adding noise: the role of noncontrastive phonetic variability in early word learning. Infancy 15(6), 608–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmale, R., Cristia, A., Seidl, A. & Johnson, E. K. (2010). Developmental changes in infants' ability to cope with dialect variation in word recognition. Infancy 15(6), 650–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmale, R., Hollich, G. & Seidl, A. (2011). Contending with foreign accent in early word learning. Journal of Child Language 38(05), 1096–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shrout, P. E. & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin 86(2), 420–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swingley, D. & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition 76(2), 147–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trudgill, P. (1999). The dialects of England (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Weinberger, S. H. (2003). Speech accent archive. George Mason University. Online: <http://accent.gmu.edu>..>Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Cohen, L. B., Lloyd, V. L., Casasola, M. & Stager, C. L. (1998). Acquisition of word−object associations by 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology 34, 1289–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, K. S. & Aslin, R. N. (2011). Adaptation to novel accents by toddlers. Developmental Science 14(2), 372–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed