Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T01:03:40.078Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A matter of meaning: reflections on forty years of JCL*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 July 2014

KATHERINE NELSON*
Affiliation:
City University of New York Graduate Center

Abstract

The entry into language via first words and, the acquisition of word meanings is considered from the perspective of publications in the Journal of Child Language over the past forty years. Problems in achieving word meanings include the disparate and sparse concepts available to the child from past prelanguage experience. Variability in beginning word learning and in its progress along a number of dimensions suggests the problems that children may encounter, as well as the strategies and styles they adopt to make progress. Social context and adult practices are vitally involved in the success of this process. Whereas much headway has been made over the past decades, much remains to be revealed through dynamic systems theory and developmental semiotic analyses, as well as laboratory research aimed at social context conditions.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Address for correspondence: Katherine Nelson, 50 Riverside Drive #4B, New York, NY 10024; tel: 212-724-1438; e-mail: [email protected]

References

REFERENCES

Akhtar, Nameera (2004). Contexts of early word learning. In Hall, D. G. & Waxman, S. R. (eds), Weaving a lexicon, 485508. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anglin, Jeremy (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language 6, 183200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, Lois (1993). The transition from infancy to language: acquiring the power of expression. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Lightbown, P. & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 40(2).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004). Myths of word learning. In Hall, D. G. & Waxman, S. R. (eds), Weaving a lexicon, 205–24.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Blum-Kulka, S. & Snow, C. E. (eds) (2002). Talking to adults: the contribution of multiparty discourse to language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganization processes in lexical and syntactic development. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., Snyder, L. & Bates, E. (1983). Individual differences at 20 months: analytic and holistic strategies in language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10, 293320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child's talk: learning to use language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1982). Semantic development: the state of the art. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art (pp. 347389). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J. & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59(5), 1173.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: an evolutionary perspective. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (eds), The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, 391428. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Furrow, D. & Nelson, K. (1984). Environmental correlates of individual differences in language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 11(3), 523–34.Google Scholar
Furrow, D. & Nelson, K. (1986). A further look at the motherese hypothesis: a reply to Gleitman, Newport & Gleitman. Journal of Child Language 13, 163–76.Google Scholar
Furrow, D., Nelson, K. & Benedict, H. (1979). Mother's speech to children and children's language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 6, 423–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampson, J. & Nelson, K. (1993). The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 20, 313–42.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M. & Hollich, G. (2000). An emergentist coalition model for word learning: mapping words to objects is a product of the interaction of multiple cues. In Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M. in collaboration with Brand, R. J., Brown, E., Chung, H. L., Hennon, E., & Rocroi, C. (eds), Becoming a word learner: a debate on lexical acquisition, 136–64. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J. & Nelson, K. (1984). Play with language: overextension as analogies. Journal of Child Language 11, 337–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, E. & Nelson, K. (1994). Words in discourse: a dialectical approach to the acquisition of meaning. Journal of Child Language 21, 367–90.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Dresner Barnes, H. (1992). Individual differences in early vocabulary development redefining the referential−expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language 19, 287310.Google Scholar
Lucariello, J. & Nelson, K. (1986). Context effects on lexical specificity in maternal and child discourse. Journal of Child Language 13, 507–22.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (ed.) (1999). The emergence of language. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind: origins of conceptual thought. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. & McDonough, L. (2000). Advancing downward to the basic level. Journal of Cognition and Development 1, 379405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (2006). Varieties of meaning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Society for Research in Child Development Monographs 38 (1/2, Serial No. 149).Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word, and sentence: interrelations in acquisition and development. Psychological Review 81, 267–85.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1979). Features, contrasts and the FCH: some comments on Barrett's lexical development hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 6, 139–46.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: the acquisition of shared meaning. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: the emergence of the mediated mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2001). Review of Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (eds), Language acquisition and conceptual development. Journal of Child Language 28, 805–15.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., Hampson, J. & Kessler-Shaw, L. (1993). The noun bias in early lexicons: evidence, explanations, and implications. Journal of Child Language 20, 6184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, A. (1983). The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975). The meaning of meaning. In Putnam, H. (ed.), Philosophical papers, Vol. 2: mind, language and reality, 215–71. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. A. (1980). Overextension in early language development. Journal of child language 7, 321–35.Google Scholar
Ross, G., Nelson, K., Wetstone, H. & Tanouye, E. (1986). Acquisition and generalization of novel object concepts by young language learners. Journal of Child Language 13, 6784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L. B. (2013). It's all connected: pathways in visual object recognition and early noun learning. American Psychologist 68, 618–29.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1999). Social perspectives on the emergence of language. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 257276). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2002). Could we please lose the mapping metaphor, please? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, 1119–20.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2008). The origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language (translated, revised, and edited by Kozulin, A.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (2004). Infants’ use of action knowledge to get a grasp on words. In Hall, D. G. & Waxman, S. R. (eds), Weaving a lexicon, 149–72. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar