Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T03:19:55.181Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Interrelations between communicative behaviors at the outset of speech: parents as observers*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 January 2010

ESTHER DROMI*
Affiliation:
Tel Aviv University
ANAT ZAIDMAN-ZAIT
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia
*
Address for correspondence: Professor Esther Dromi, Constantiner School of Education, Tel Aviv University, Ramat Aviv, Israel, 69978. fax: 972-3-648-0249; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The Hebrew Parent Questionnaire for Communication and Early Language (HPQ-CEL) was administered by 154 parents of Hebrew-speaking toddlers aged 1 ; 0 to 1 ; 3 (77 boys, 77 girls). The Questionnaire guided parents in observing and rating their toddlers in six contexts at home. The study aimed to identify inter-correlations between toddlers' non-linguistic behaviors that co-occur during the transition to speech. Seven communicative behaviors were extracted from the questionnaire data: Crying, Vocalizations, Collaboration with Adults, Pointing, Words, Joint Engagement in a Peek-a-Boo Game, and Triadic Interaction in Book Reading. Collaboration with Adults and Triadic Interaction in Book Reading yielded more significant correlations than other prelinguistic behaviors. Participation in social games and book-reading activities was associated with the toddlers' number of words at the period studied.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We wish to mention with gratitude our late Italian colleague Dr Luigia Camaioni, who developed with her colleagues the Parent Questionnaire and gave us permission to use it in our research program. The data reported herein were collected in three separate studies: (1) Quality of Early Childcare at Haifa University, directed by Professor Avi Sagi-Schwartz and funded by a grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (RO1 #HD25975); and (2) The MA dissertations of Dorit Shalom and of Anat Zaidman-Zait at Tel Aviv University. We thank Dorit Shalom, Dalia Ringwald–Frimerman, Esther Guralnik, Hadas Treitel and Julia Reznik for participation in the adaptation of the HPQ-CEL to Hebrew. Thanks are also extended to Dee B. Ankonina for her editorial help.

References

REFERENCES

Adamson, L. B. (1995). Communication development during infancy. Madison: Brown & Benchmark.Google Scholar
Adamson, L. B. & Chance, S. E. (1998). Coordinating attention to people, ‘objects’ and language. In Wetherby, A. M., Warren, S. F. & Reichle, J. (eds), Transitions in prelinguistic communication, 1538. Baltimore: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
Ben-Itzhak, D. & Levin, I. (1995). Preverbal communication and play development of hearing impaired and hearing infants. Paper presented at the VIIIth International Congress for the Study of Child Language, University of the Basque Country, San Sebastian, Spain.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9, 5572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L., Caselli, M. C., Longobardi, E. & Volterra, V. (1991). A parent report instrument for early language assessment. First Language 11, 345–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L., Caselli, M. C., Volterra, V. & Luchenti, S. (1992). Questionario sullo sviluppo comunicativo e linguistico nel secondo anno di vita. Firenze: Organizzazioni Specialu.Google Scholar
Camaioni, L. & Laicardi, C. (1985). Early social games and the acquisition of language. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 3, 3139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caprici, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizutto, E. & Volterra, V. (1996). Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language 23(3), 645–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communication competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 63(4, Serial No. 255).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselli, M. C. (1990). Communicative gestures and first words. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children, 5667. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V. (1990). From communication to language in hearing and deaf children. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children, 263–72. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dromi, E. (2002). Babbling and early words. In Salkind, N. J. & Margolis, L. (eds), Child development: Macmillan psychology reference series, Vol. 1, 4547. New York: Macmillan Reference.Google Scholar
Dromi, E. (2003). Assessment of prelinguistic behaviors in deaf children: Parents as collaborators. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 8(4), 367–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dromi, E., Ben-Shahar-Treitel, H., Guralnik, E. & Ringwald-Frimerman, D. (1992). The Hebrew adaptation for deaf children of the Camaioni et al. (1992) parent report instrument for assessing communicative and linguistic development in the second year of life. Unpublished manuscript, Tel Aviv University [Hebrew].Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, E. & Pethick, J. P. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59(5, Serial No. 242).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwyn, S., Acredolo, L. & Brown, C. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24, 81–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth, and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6, 1940.Google Scholar
Mundy, P. & Gomez, A. (1998). Individual differences in joint attention skill development in the second year of life. Infant Behavior and Development 21, 469–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Kasari, C., Sigman, M. & Ruskin, E. (1995). Nonverbal communication and early language acquisition in children with Down syndrome and in normally developing children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 157–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadel, J. & Camaioni, L. (eds)1993. New perspectives in early communicative development. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Reddy, V. (1999). Prelinguistic communication. In Barrett, M. (ed.), Early lexical development, 2550. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ringwald-Frimerman, D. (2003). From communication to language in two linguistic environments: Speaking and signing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Tel Aviv University [Hebrew].Google Scholar
Sagi, A., Koren-Karie, N., Gini, M., Ziv, Y. & Joels, T. (2002). Shedding further light on the effects of various types and quality of early childcare on infant–mother attachment relationship: The Haifa study of early childcare. Child Development 73(4), 1166–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, R. & Leonard, L. (1982). Do children pick and choose? An examination of phonological selection and avoidance in early lexical acquisition. Journal of Child Language 9, 319–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, L. (1982). Reproductive, perinatal, and environmental factors as predictors of the cognitive and language developments of preterm and full term infants. Child Development 53, 963–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinai-Carmona, G., Oren, A. & Levin-Epstein, N. (2007). Internet surveys: A new era in survey research. Lucille Cohen Institute for Public Research Quarterly 6, 58 [Hebrew].Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1998). Role of babbling and phonology in early linguistic development. In Wetherby, A. M., Warren, S. F. & Reichle, J. (eds), Transitions in prelinguistic communication, 87–110. Baltimore: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
Sugarman, S. (1983). Empirical versus logical issues in the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. In Golinkoff, R. M. (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication, 133–45. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as a social cognition. In Moore, C. E. & Dunhum, P. J. (eds), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development, 103131. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57, 1457–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veneziano, E. (1988). Vocal–verbal interaction and the construction of early lexical knowledge. In Smith, M. D. & Locke, J. L. (eds), The emergent lexicon: The child's development of a linguistic vocabulary, 109147. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1996). Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Zinober, B. & Martlew, M. (1985). Developmental changes in four types of gestures in relation to acts and vocalizations from 10 to 21 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 3, 293306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar