Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T07:00:32.704Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How to make sense of temporal/spatial ‘before’ and ‘after’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Lynne Feagans
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Abstract

Two experiments were devised in order to better understand the relationship between temporal before and after and their spatial counterparts. In Experiment I, sixty adults were probed for their spatial metaphoric understanding of temporal before and after. Although H. Clark proposed that temporal before and after were derived from their spatial counterparts, Experiment I did not support that notion. In fact, the subjects overwhelmingly reported that temporal before was more related to spatial after and that temporal after was more related to spatial before. Since it has been shown by numerous investigators that temporal before is acquired before temporal after by children, the evidence from Experiment I suggested that their spatial relatives may be acquired similarly. Experiment II presents evidence that indeed spatial after is understood better by three-year-old children than spatial before. This evidence is coincident with the adult reports in Experiment I.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ames, I. B. (1948). The development of verbalized space in the young child. JGenetPsychol 72. 6384.Google ScholarPubMed
Amidon, A. & Carey, P. (1972). Why five-year-olds cannot understand before and after. JVLVB 11. 153–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asso, D. & Wyke, M. (1973). Verbal descriptions of spatial relations in line drawings by children. BJPsychol 64. 233–40.Google ScholarPubMed
Barrie-Blackley, S. (1973). Six-year-old children's understanding of sentences adjoined with time adverbs. JPsycholingRes 2. 153–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (1970). How young children describe events in time. In d'Arcais, C. B. F. & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds), Advances in psycholinguistics. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1971). On the acquisition of the meaning of before and after. JVLVB 10. 266–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time and semantics and the child. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Feagans, L. (1980). Children's understanding of some temporal terms, denoting order, duration and simultaneity. JPsycholingRes 9. 4157.Google ScholarPubMed
Friedman, W. J. & Seely, P. B. (1976). The child's acquisition of spatial and temporal word meanings. ChDev 47. 1103–8.Google Scholar
Harris, L. & Strommen, E. (1972). The role of front-back features in children's ‘front’, ‘back’ and ‘beside’ placements of objects. MPQ 18. 259–71.Google Scholar
Johnson, H. (1975). The meaning of before and after in preschool children. JExpChPsychol 19. 88–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Keller-Cohen, D. (1975). Children's verbal imitation, comprehension, and production of temporal structures. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. & Maratsos, M. P. (1975). On the acquisition of front, back, and side. ChDev 46. 202–10.Google Scholar