Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T10:12:30.524Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exposure and feedback in language acquisition: adult construals of children's early verb-form use in Hebrew

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 October 2018

Lyle LUSTIGMAN*
Affiliation:
Department of Communicative Disorders and Sciences, San Jose State University
Eve V. CLARK
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Stanford University
*
*Corresponding author: Department of Communicative Disorders and Sciences, San Jose State University, San Jose, CA 95192, USA. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study focuses on adult responses to children's verb uses, the information they provide, and how they change over time. We analyzed longitudinal samples from four children acquiring Hebrew (age-range: 1;4–2;5; child verb-forms = 8,337). All child verbs were coded for inflectional category, and for whether and how adults responded to them. Our findings show that: (a) children's early verbs were opaque with no clear inflectional target (e.g., the child-form tapes corresponds to letapes ‘to-climb’, metapes ‘is-climbing’, yetapes ‘will-climb’), with inflections added gradually; (b) most early verbs were followed by adult responses using the same lexeme; and (c) as opacity in children's verbs decreased, adults made fewer uses of the same lexeme in their responses, and produced a broader array of inflections and inflectional shifts. In short, adults are attuned to what their children know and respond to their early productions accordingly, with extensive ‘tailor-made’ feedback on their verb uses.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adam, G., & Bat-El, O. (2008). Segmental effects on syllable selection: Evidence from Hebrew. In Gavarró, A. & Freitas, M. J. (Eds.), Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA 2007 (pp. 111). Newcastle, UK: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S. (1996). The minimalist child: parameters and functional heads in the acquisition of Hebrew. PhD dissertation, Tel-Aviv University.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., & Berman, R. A. (2003). The emergence of grammar: Early verbs and beyond. Journal of Child Language, 30, 845877.Google Scholar
Ashkenazi, O., Ravid, D., & Gillis, S. (2016). Breaking into the Hebrew verb system: A learning problem. First Language, 36, 505524.Google Scholar
Bassano, D. (2000). Early development of nouns and verbs in French: Exploring the interface between lexicon and grammar. Journal of Child Language, 27, 521559.Google Scholar
Bat-El, O. (2002). Semitic verb structure within a universal perspective. In Shimron, J. (Ed.), Language processing and acquisition in languages of Semitic, root-based morphology (pp. 2959). Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bat-El, O. (2012). Phonological constraints on morphological development: The acquisition of Hebrew verb inflectional suffixes. Brill's Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics, 4, 189212.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1978). Early verbs: Comments on how and why a child uses his first words. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 5, 2139.Google Scholar
Berman, R.A., (1982). Verb-pattern alternation: The interface of morphology, syntax, and semantics in Hebrew child language. Journal of Child Language, 9, 169191.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1990). Acquiring an (S)VO language: Subjectless sentences in children's Hebrew. Linguistics, 28, 11351166.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1993). Marking of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 20, 641669.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Armon-Lotem, S. (1997). How grammatical are early verbs? In Martinot, C. (Ed.), Actes du colloque international sur l'acquisition de la syntaxe en langue maternelle et en language étrangère (pp. 1760). Besançon, FR: Annales Littéraires de l'Université de Franche-Comté, numéro 631.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Lustigman, L. (2012). HARSP: A developmental language profile for Hebrew. In Ball, M. J., Crystal, D., & Fletcher, P. (Eds.), Assessing grammar: The languages of LARSP (pp. 4376). Bristol, UK: Bristol Language Series.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. & Lustigman, L. (2014). Emergent clause-combining in adult–child interactional contexts, In Arnon, I., Casillas, M., Kurumada, C., & Estigarribia, B. (Eds.), Language in interaction: studies in honor of Eve V. Clark (pp. 281300). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Neeman, Y. (1994). Development of linguistic forms: Hebrew. In Berman, R. A. & Slobin, D. I. (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental dtudy (pp. 285328). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1991). Language development from two to three. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Lifter, K., & Hafitz, J. (1980). Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language. Language, 56, 386412.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1985). What shapes children's grammars? In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition (vol. 2, pp. 12571319). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brown, P. (1998). Children's first verbs in Tzeltal: Evidence for an early verb category. Linguistics, 36, 713754.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Chouinard, M. M., & Clark, E. V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language, 30, 637669.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1990). On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 17, 417431.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2016). First language acquisition (3rd edn). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V., & de Marneffe, M.-C. (2012). Constructing verb paradigms in French: adult construals and emerging grammatical contrasts. Morphology, 22, 89120.Google Scholar
de León, L. (1999a). Verbs in Tzotzil (Mayan) early syntactic development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 3, 219239.Google Scholar
de León, L. (1999b). Verb roots and caregiver speech in early Tzotzil (Mayan) acquisition. In Fox, B., Jurafsky, D., & Michaelis, L. A. (Eds.), Cognition and function in language (pp. 99119). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G. (1985). Learning how to use verbs: Lexical coding and the influence of the input. Journal of Child Language, 12, 587595.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 267278.Google Scholar
Farrar, M. J. (1990). Discourse and the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language, 17, 607624.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., Sebastián, E., & Soto, P. (1999). The early acquisition of Spanish verbal morphology: Across-the-board or piecemeal knowledge? International Journal of Bilingualism, 3, 133182.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (1978). On relational meaning: The acquisition of verb meaning. Child Development, 49, 988998.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relativity, and early word learning. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 215256). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Göksun, T., Aktan-Erciyes, A., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Event perception and language learning: Early interactions between language and thought. In Nihan Ketrez, E., Küntay, A., Özçaliʂkan, S., & Özyürek, A. (Eds.), Social environment and cognition in language development: Studies in honor of Ayhan Aksu-Koç (pp. 179198). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hadley, P. A., & Holt, J. K. (2006). Individual differences in the onset of tense marking: a growth curve analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 9841000.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. (2008). Building language competence in first language acquisition. European Review, 16, 445456.Google Scholar
Lustigman, L. (2012). Non-finiteness in early Hebrew verbs. Brill's Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics, 4, 213231.Google Scholar
Lustigman, L. (2013). Developing structural specification: Productivity in early Hebrew verb usage. First Language, 33, 4767.Google Scholar
Lustigman, L. (2015). Optional opacity: A syntactically-based analysis of early Hebrew verb morphology. First Language, 35, 466488.Google Scholar
Lustigman, L. (2016). Interfaces between linguistic systems: Evidence from child language. Linguistics, 54, 273303.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1975). Rules, rote, and analogy in morphological formations by Hungarian children. Journal of Child Language, 2, 6677.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2005). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, vol. 1: The format and programs (3rd edn). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pizzuto, E., & Caselli, M. C. (1994). The acquisition of Italian verb morphology in a cross-linguistic perspective. In Levy, Y. (Ed.), Other children, other languages: Issues in the theory of language acquisition (pp. 137187). Milton Park, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. (1997). Morphological development a duo: pre- and proto-morphology in Hebrew-speaking twins. Papers and Studies in Contrastic Linguistics, 33, 79102.Google Scholar
Ravid, D., Ashkenazi, O., Levie, R., Ben Zadok, G., Grunwald, T., Bratslavsky, R. and Gillis, S., (2016). Foundations of the early root category: Analyses of linguistic input to Hebrew-speaking children. In Berman, R. A. (Ed.), Acquisition and development of Hebrew (pp. 95134). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Richards, B. J. (1990). Language Development and Individual Differences: A Study of Auxiliary Verb Learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rispoli, M., Hadley, P. A., & Holt, J. K. (2009). The growth of tense productivity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 930944.Google Scholar
Rispoli, M., Hadley, P. A., & Holt, J. K. (2012). Sequence and system in the acquisition of tense and agreement. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 10071021.Google Scholar
Rojas Nieto, C. (2011). Developing first contrasts in Spanish verb inflection: usage and interaction. In Arnon, I. & Clark, E. V. (Eds.), Experience, variation, and generalization: Learning a first language (pp. 5372). Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Fletcher, S. L., & Freudenthal, D. (2008). Repetition and reuse in child language learning. In Behrens, H. (Ed.), Corpora in language acquisition research: History, methods, perspectives (pp. 124). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Stahl, D. (2004). Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language, 31, 101121.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. (1999). Early lexical, morphological and syntactic development in French: Some complex relations. International Journal of Bilingualism, 3, 183217.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. (2014). Conversation and language acquisition: Unique properties and effects. In Arnon, I., Casillas, M., Kurumada, C., & Estigarribia, B. (Eds.), Language in interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark (pp. 83100). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E., & Clark, E. V. (2016). Early verb constructions in French: Adjacency on the left edge. Journal of Child Language, 43, 11931230.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E., & Parisse, C. (2010). The acquisition of early verbs in French: Assessing the role of conversation and child-directed speech. First Language, 30, 287311.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., & Vija, M. (2006). The acquisition of verbal inflection in Estonian: Two case studies. In Gagarina, N. & Gülzow, I. (Eds.), The acquisition of verbs and their grammar: The effect of particular languages (pp. 263295). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Xanthos, A., & Gillis, S. (2010). Quantifying the development of inflectional diversity. First Language, 30, 175198.Google Scholar