Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Tomasello, Michael
and
Akhtar, Nameera
1995.
Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions.
Cognitive Development,
Vol. 10,
Issue. 2,
p.
201.
Saxon, Terrill F.
1997.
A longitudinal study of early mother-infant interaction and later language competence.
First Language,
Vol. 17,
Issue. 51,
p.
271.
Rollins, Pamela Rosenthal
Wambacq, Ilse
Dowell, Debbie
Mathews, Lauren
and
Reese, Pam Britton
1998.
An intervention technique for children with autistic spectrum disorder: joint attentional routines.
Journal of Communication Disorders,
Vol. 31,
Issue. 2,
p.
181.
Saxon, Terrill F.
and
Reilly, John T.
1998.
Language Competence and Joint Attention in Mother‐Toddler Dyads.
Early Child Development and Care,
Vol. 142,
Issue. 1,
p.
33.
Samuelson, Larissa K.
and
Smith, Linda B.
1998.
Memory and Attention Make Smart Word Learning: An Alternative Account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello.
Child Development,
Vol. 69,
Issue. 1,
p.
94.
Gelman, Susan A
and
Ebeling, Karen S
1998.
Shape and representational status in children's early naming.
Cognition,
Vol. 66,
Issue. 2,
p.
B35.
Carpenter, Malinda
Akhtar, Nameera
and
Tomasello, Michael
1998.
Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions.
Infant Behavior and Development,
Vol. 21,
Issue. 2,
p.
315.
Rollins, Pamela Rosenthal
1999.
Early Pragmatic Accomplishments and Vocabulary Development in Preschool Children With Autism.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
Vol. 8,
Issue. 2,
p.
181.
Tomasello, Michael
1999.
The Human Adaptation for Culture.
Annual Review of Anthropology,
Vol. 28,
Issue. 1,
p.
509.
Bedore, Lisa M.
and
Leonard, Laurence B.
2000.
The Effects of Inflectional Variation on Fast Mapping of Verbs in English and Spanish.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
Vol. 43,
Issue. 1,
p.
21.
Baldwin, Dare A.
2000.
Interpersonal Understanding Fuels Knowledge Acquisition.
Current Directions in Psychological Science,
Vol. 9,
Issue. 2,
p.
40.
Johnson, Susan C.
2000.
The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Vol. 4,
Issue. 1,
p.
22.
Fonagy, Peter
Target, Mary
and
Gergely, George
2000.
ATTACHMENT AND BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America,
Vol. 23,
Issue. 1,
p.
103.
Baldwin, Dare A.
and
Baird, Jodie A.
2001.
Discerning intentions in dynamic human action.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Vol. 5,
Issue. 4,
p.
171.
Chiat, Shula
2001.
Mapping theories of developmental language impairment: Premises, predictions and evidence.
Language and Cognitive Processes,
Vol. 16,
Issue. 2-3,
p.
113.
Tomasello, Michael
2001.
Cultural Transmission.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
Vol. 32,
Issue. 2,
p.
135.
Diesendruck, Gil
and
Markson, Lori
2001.
Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account..
Developmental Psychology,
Vol. 37,
Issue. 5,
p.
630.
Rakison, David H.
and
Poulin-Dubois, Diane
2001.
Developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinction..
Psychological Bulletin,
Vol. 127,
Issue. 2,
p.
209.
O'Neill, Daniela K.
Topolovec, Jane
and
Stern-Cavalcante, Wilma
2002.
Feeling Sponginess: The Importance of Descriptive Gestures in 2- and 3-Year-Old Children's Acquisition of Adjectives.
Journal of Cognition and Development,
Vol. 3,
Issue. 3,
p.
243.
Swingley, Daniel
and
Fernald, Anne
2002.
Recognition of Words Referring to Present and Absent Objects by 24-Month-Olds.
Journal of Memory and Language,
Vol. 46,
Issue. 1,
p.
39.