Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T12:11:25.187Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early bilingual development: one language or two?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Fred Genesee*
Affiliation:
McGill University
*
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Stewart Biological Sciences Building, 1205 Dr Penfield Avenue, Montreal, QC, H3A 1B1, Canada.

Abstract

It is commonly thought that children learning two languages simultaneously during infancy go through a stage when they cannot differentiate their two languages. Virtually all studies of infant bilingual development have found that bilingual children mix elements from their two languages. These results have been interpreted as evidence for a unitary, undifferentiated language system (the unitary language system hypothesis). The empirical basis for these claims is re-examined and it is argued that, contrary to most extant interpretations, bilingual children develop differentiated language systems from the beginning and are able to use their developing languages in contextually sensitive ways. A call for more serious attention to the possible role of parental input in the form of mixed utterances is made.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

I would like to thank the following people for helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper: Naomi Goodz, Naomi Holobow, Nina Spada, Jack Upshur and Lydia White.

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Balken, L. (1970). Les effets du bilinguisme français-anglais sur les aptitudes intellectuelles. Bruxelles: Aimav.Google Scholar
Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive development and cognitive strategy. Child Development 48. 1009–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergman, C. R. (1976). Interference vs. Independent development in infant bilingualism. In Keller, G. D., Taeschner, R. V. & Viera, S. (eds), Bilingualism in the bicentennial and beyond. New York: Bilingual Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burling, R. (1978). Language development of a Garo and English-speaking child. In Hatch, E. (ed.), Second language acquisition: a book of readings. Rowley MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1976). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive growth: a synthesis of research findings and explanatory hypotheses. Working Papers on Bilingualism 9. 143.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In Schooling and language minority students: a theoretical framework. Los Angeles: Evaluation, Dissemination, and Assessment Center.Google Scholar
Diaz, R. (1983). Thought and two languages: the impact of bilingualism on cognitive development. In Norbeck, E., Price-Williams, D. & McCord, W. (eds), Review of Research in Education. Vol. 10. Washington DC: American Educational Research Association.Google Scholar
Fantini, A. E. (1978). Bilingual behavior and social cues: case studies of two bilingual children. In Paradis, M. (ed.), Aspects of bilingualism. Columbia SC: Hornbeam Press.Google Scholar
Garcia, E. E. (1983). Early childhood bilingualism. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Genesee, F. (1987). Learning through two languages: studies of immersion and bilingual education. Cambridge MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Goodluck, H. (1987). Language acquisition and linguistic theory. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Second edition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Goodz, N. S. (1986). Parental language in bilingual families: a model and some data. Paper presented at the Third Congress of the World Association of Infant Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Stockholm, Sweden.Google Scholar
Goodz, N. S. (in press). Parental language mixing in bilingual families. Journal of Infant Mental Health 10.Google Scholar
Griffiths, P. (1986). Early vocabulary. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Second edition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Hakuta, K. (1986). Mirror of language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hatch, E. (1978). Second language acquisition: a book of readings. Rowley MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Ianco-Worrall, A. D. (1972). Bilingualism and cognitive development. Child Development 43. 1390–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imedadze, N. (1978). On the psychological nature of child speech formation under conditions of exposure to two languages. In Hatch, E. (ed.), Second language acquisition: a book of readings. Rowley MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. (1981). Infant speech perception: a critical appraisal. In Eimas, P. D. & Miller, J. L. (eds), Perspectives on the study of speech. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. (1982). Auditory versus phonetic coding of speech signals during infancy. In Mehler, J., Walker, E. & Garrett, M. (eds), Perspectives on mental representation. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. F. (1949). Speech development of a bilingual child: a linguist's record. Vol. 4. Grammar and general problems. Evanston ILL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. (1978). A child's learning of two languages. In Hatch, E. (ed.), Second language acquisition: a book of readings. Rowley MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Lindholm, K. J. & Padilla, A. M. (1978). Language mixing in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language 5. 327–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, B. (1978). Second language acquisition in childhood. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mehler, J., Bertoncini, J., Barrière, M. & Jassik-Gerschenfeld, D. (1978). Infant recognition of mother's voice. Perception 7. 491–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehler, J., Lambertz, G., Jusczyk, P. & Amiel-Tison, C. (1986). Discrimination de la langue maternelle par le nouveau-né. Académie des Sciences 3. 637–40.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. (1989). Early differentiation of languages in bilingual children. In Hyltenstam, K. & Obler, L. (eds), Bilingualism across the lifespan: aspects of acquisition, maturity and loss. Cambridge MA: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Murrell, M. (1966). Language acquisition in a trilingual environment: notes from a case-study. Studia Linguistica 20. 935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oksaar, E. (1971). Code switching as an interactional strategy for developing bilingual competence. Word 27. 377–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padilla, A. M. & Liebman, E. (1975). Language acquisition in the bilingual child. Bilingual Review 2. 3455.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. New York: Atheneum.Google Scholar
Poplack, S. (1979). Sometimes I'll start a sentence in Spanish y termino en Espanol: toward a typology of code-switching. Working Paper No. 4. New York: Centro de Estudios Puertorriquenos.Google Scholar
Pye, C. (1986). One lexicon or two? An alternative interpretation of early bilingual speech. Journal of Child Language 13. 591–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redlinger, W. E. & Park, T. (1980). Language mixing in young bilinguals. Journal of Child Language 7. 337–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reich, P. (1976). The early acquisition of word meaning. Journal of Child Language 3. 117–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronjat, J. (1913). Le développement du langage observé chez un enfant bilingue. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Sridhar, S. N. & Sridhar, K. K. (1980). The syntax and psycholinguistics of bilingual code mixing. Canadian Journal of Psychology 34. 407–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stross, B. (1973). Acquisition of botanical terminology by Tzeltal children. In Edmondson, M. S. (ed.), Meaning in Mayan languages. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Swain, M. (1972). Bilingualism as a first language. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Irvine.Google Scholar
Swain, M. (1977). Bilingualism, monolingualism and code acquisition. In Mackey, W. & Andersson, T. (eds), Bilingualism in early childhood. Rowley MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Swain, M. & Wesche, M. (1975). Linguistic interaction: case study of a bilingual child. Language Sciences 17. 1722.Google Scholar
Tabouret-Keller, A. (1962). Vrais et faux problèmes du bilinguisme. In Cohen, M., Rezine, J., Kocher, F., Brauner, A., Lentin, L. & Tabouret-Keller, A. (eds), Etudes sur le langage de l'enfant. Paris: Les Editions du Scarabée.Google Scholar
Trehub, S. (1973). Auditory-linguistic sensitivity in infants. Ph.D. dissertation, McGill University, Montreal.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. (1982). The acquisition of morphology by a bilingual child: a whole-word approach. Applied Psycholinguistics 3. 141–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. (1985). Language differentiation by the bilingual infant. Journal of Child Language 12. 297324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vihman, M. & McLaughlin, B. (1982). Bilingualism and second language acquisition in preschool children. In Brainerd, C. & Pressley, M., Verbal processes in children. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Volterra, V. & Taeschner, T. (1978). The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language 5. 311–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar