Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T16:02:56.108Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The development of the causative construction in Persian child language*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 March 2015

NEILOUFAR FAMILY*
Affiliation:
University of Kaiserslautern
SHANLEY E. M. ALLEN
Affiliation:
University of Kaiserslautern
*
Address for correspondence: Neiloufar Family, University of Kaiserslautern – Faculty of Social Sciences, Kaiserslautern, Germany. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The acquisition of systematic patterns and exceptions in different languages can be readily examined using the causative construction. Persian allows four types of causative structures, including one productive multiword structure (i.e. the light verb construction). In this study, we examine the development of all four structures in Persian child speech between the ages of 1;11 and 6;7, in correspondence with their caregivers’ speech. We define developmental stages based on dendrograms derived from variability clustering (Gries & Stoll, 2009). These stages are further substantiated by qualitative data, including overgeneralization errors and alternating structures. We find that Persian-speaking children learn to exploit two (i.e. lexical and light verb construction causatives) of the four constructions. They go from relying on lexical causatives to forming progressively constrained templates for the more complex light verb construction. This first study of the development of Persian causatives supports a usage-based account of verb-by-verb learning in child language development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was made possible through support by the ANR (Agence Nationale de la Recherche) project COLAJE (Paris, France) and the Institute for Cognitive Science Studies (Tehran, Iran). We would also like to thank Stefan Gries for providing the tools for the statistical analyses used in this paper and Laleh Ghadakpour for cross-checking the data coding in the corpus.

References

REFERENCES

Allen, S. E. M. (1998). Categories within the verb category: learning the causative in Inuktitut. Linguistics 36(4), 633–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, S. E. M. & Crago, M. (1996). Early passive acquisition in Inuktitut. Journal of Child Language 23, 129–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Argus, R., Uziel-Karl, S. & Laalo, K. (2011). Causative-formation: a comparative perspective. Poster presented at the International Association for the Study of Child Language Meeting, 18–24 July, 2011, Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
Berman, R. (1982). Verb-pattern alternation: the interface of morphology, syntax and semantics in Hebrew. Journal of Child Language 9, 169–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, R. (1986). A step-by-step model of language learning. In Levin, I. (ed.), Stage and structure: re-opening the debate (pp. 191219). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Berman, R. (1994). Developmental perspectives on transitivity: a confluence of cues. In Levy, Y. (ed.), Other children, other languages: issues in the theory of language acquisition (pp. 189263). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1974). Learning the structure of causative verbs: a study in the relationship of cognitive, semantic, and syntactic development. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 8, 142–78.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Evaluating competing linguistic models with language acquisition data: implications of developmental errors. Quaderni di Semantica 3(1), 566.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. & Croft, W. (2008). The acquisition of the English causative alternation. In Bowerman, M. & Brown, P. (eds), Crosslinguistic perspectives on argument structure: implications for learnability (pp. 279306). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brooks, P. & Tomasello, M. (1999). How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language 75, 720–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, J. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 1153). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Butt, M. & Geuder, W. (2001). Light verbs in Urdu and grammaticalisation. In Eckardt, R., von Heusinger, K. & Schwarze, C. (eds), Words in time: diachronic semantics from different points of view (pp. 295350). Berlin: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Comrie, B. (1985). Causative verb formation and other verb-deriving morphology. In Shopen, T. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, vol. 3 (pp. 309348). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Courtney, E. H. (2002). Child acquisition of Quechua causatives and change-of-state verbs. First Language 22, 2971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, W. (2003). Typology and universals, 2nd ed., (xixxxv). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (2008). German children's comprehension of word order and case marking in causative sentences. Child Development 79(4), 1152–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Family, N. (2006). Explorations of semantic space: the case of light verb constructions in Persian. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Ecole des Hautes Etudes en Sciences Sociales.Google Scholar
Family, N. (2009). Lighten up: the acquisition of light verb constructions in Persian. In BUCLD Proceedings 33, Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Folli, R., Harley, H. & Karimi, S. (2004). Determinants of event type in Persian complex predicates. Cambridge Occasional Papers in Linguistics 1, 101–25.Google Scholar
Göksun, T., Küntay, A. & Naigles, L. (2008). Turkish children use morphosyntactic bootstrapping in interpreting verb meaning. Journal of Child Language, 35, 291323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golfam, A. & Dehghan, M. (2012). On type: the so-called causativization in Persian. Theory and Practice in Language Studies 2(7), 1536–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. & Stoll, S. (2009). Finding developmental groups in acquisition data: variability-based neighbor clustering. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics 16(3), 217–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, P. & Thompson, S. A. (1980). Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language 56, 251–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imai, M., Haryu, E., Okada, H., Kajikawa, S. & Saalbach, H. (2007). Case-marking and argument-number dilemma in children learning an argument dropping language in inferring novel verb meanings. Paper presented at the SRCD Biennial Meeting, Boston, USA.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1990). Semantic structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lehmann, Christian (1982). Directions for interlinear morphemic translations. Folia Linguistica 16, 199224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lidz, J., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (2003). Understanding how input matters: verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar. Cognition 87, 151–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C. (1979). “Don't you fall me down”: children's generalizations regarding cause and transitivity. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development (Stanford University Department of Linguistics) 17, 81–9.Google Scholar
Lotfi, A. (2008). Causative constructions in modern Persian. California Linguistic Notes 33(2), 131.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. (1979). How to get from words to sentences. In Aaronson, D. & Reiber, R. (eds), Psycholinguistic research: implications and applications (pp. 285354). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Megerdoomian, K. (2002). Beyond words and phrases: a unified theory of predicate composition. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Southern California.Google Scholar
Mohanan, T. (1996). Multidimensionality of representation: NV complex predicates in Hindi. In Alsina, A., Bresnan, J. & Sells, P. (eds), Complex predicates (pp. 431471). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Morikawa, H. (1990). Acquisition of transitivity in Japanese early use of transitive/intransitive verb pairs. Working Papers in Language Development 5(1), 113.Google Scholar
Naigles, L., Fowler, A. & Helm, A. (1992). Developmental shifts in the construction of verb meanings. Cognitive Development 7, 403–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pye, C. (1994). A cross-linguistic approach to the causative alternation. In Levy, Y. (ed.), Other children, other languages: issues in the theory of language acquisition (pp. 243363).Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2013). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Online: <http://www.R-project.org/>..>Google Scholar
Song, J. J. (1996). Causatives and causation: a universal-typological perspective. New York, NY: Addison Wesley Longman.Google Scholar
Shibatani, M. & Pardeshi, P. (2002). The causative continuum. In Shibatani, M. (ed.), The grammar of causation and interpersonal manipulation (pp. 85126). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science 12(1), 49100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: a case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74, 209–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Stahl, D. (2004). Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language 31, 101–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ural, A. E., Yüret, D., Ketrez, F. N., Kocbas, D. & Küntay, A. (2009). Morphological cues vs. number of nominals in learning verb types in Turkish: syntactic bootstrapping mechanism revisited. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(10), 1393–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valian, V. (1986). Syntactic categories in the speech of young children. Developmental Psychology 22, 562–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hout, A. (1996). Event semantics of verb frame alternations: a case study of Dutch and its acquisition. Tilburg: Tilburg Dissertation in Language Studies.Google Scholar