Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:04:41.617Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The development of relational arcs in the lexical semantic memory structures of young children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Peter F. Norlin
Affiliation:
Vanderbilt University

Abstract

Forty-eight normally developing children, twelve each at ages three, four five, and six years, were asked to define and describe ten concrete nouns expressively with no attendant visual stimuli. Responses were coded according to the semantic relations expressed. Nineteen different semantic relationships were isolated in these definitions, and significant differences were found between the age levels in the various configurations of semantic relational categories represented in the first five relations elicited from each child by each noun. Compatible with recent hypotheses concerning the development of internal semantic memory structures, these differences also proved to be highly predictive of a subject's chronological age. The results suggest that semantic relational analysis may be a productive method of documenting the characteristics of lexical semantic systems of very young children.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, J. R. (1972). FRAN: A simulation model of free recall. In Bower, G. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 5. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human associative memory. Washington, D.C.: Winston.Google Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1978). From reference to meaning. ChDev 49. 969–76.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1973). Early syntactic development: a cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finnish. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. & Berko, J. (1960). Word association and the acquisition of grammar. ChDev 31. 114.Google ScholarPubMed
Carroll, J. B. & White, M. N. (1973). Age-of-acquisition norms for 220 picturable nouns. JVLVB 12. 563–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973). What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1974). Some aspects of the conceptual basis for first language acquisition. In Schiefelbush, R. L. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds), Language perspectives – acquisition, retardation, and intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1977). Strategies and the mapping problem in first language acquisition. In Macnamara, J. (ed.), Language learning and thought. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Garnica, O. K. (1974). Is he coming or going? On the acquisition of deictic verbs. JVLVB 13. 559–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M. & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. PsychRev 82. 407–28.Google Scholar
Collins, A. M. & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval from semantic memory. JVLVB 8. 240–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M. (1972). How to make a language user. In Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W. (eds), Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Deese, J. (1965). The structure of associations in language and thought. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. & Wales, R. (1970). On the acquisition of some relational terms. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Duncan, E. M. & Kellas, G. (1978). Developmental changes in the internal structures of semantic categories. JExpChPsych 26. 328–40.Google Scholar
Entwisle, D. R. (1966). The word associations of young children. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Entwisle, D. R., Forsyth, D. & Muus, R. (1964). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift in children's word associations. JVLVB 3. 1929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin, S. (1961). Changes with age in the verbal determinants of word association. AmJPsych 74. 361–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Feifel, H. & Lorge, I. (1950). Qualitative differences in the vocabulary responses of children. JEdPsych 41. 118.Google Scholar
Freedman, J. L. & Loftus, E. F. (1971). Retrieval of words from long-term memory. JVLVB 10. 106–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes-Roth, B. & Hayes-Roth, F. (1977). The prominence of lexical information in memory representations of meaning. JVLVB 16. 119–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1972). Notes on the structure of semantic memory. In Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W. (eds), Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1974). The representation of meaning in memory. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kruglov, L. P. (1953). Qualitative differences in the vocabulary choices of children as revealed in multiple choice test. JEdPsych 44. 229–43.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. (1975). On the acquisition of a semantic system. JVLVB 14. 340–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, S. & Maratsos, M. (1975). On the acquisition of front, back, and side. ChDev 46. 202–10.Google Scholar
Litowitz, B. (1977). Learning to make definitions. JChLang 4. 289304.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. (1972). Nouns, adjectives, and semantic memory. JExpPsych 96. 213–15.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. & Grober, E. (1973). Retrieval from semantic memory by young children. DevPsych 8. 310.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. & Cole, W. (1974). Retrieving attribute and name information from semantic memory. JExpPsych 102. 1116–22.Google Scholar
Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Mansfield, A. F. (1977). Semantic organization in the young child: evidence for the development of semantic feature systems. JExpChPsych 23. 5777.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1978). The acquisition of word meaning. ChDev 49. 9991004.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr.Soc.Res.Ch.Devel. 38 1137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word, and sentence: interrelations in acquisition and development. PsychRev 81. 267–85.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1977 a). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: a review of research and theory. PsychBull 84. 93116.Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1977 b). The conceptual basis for naming. In Macnamara, J. (ed.), Language learning and thought. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1978). Semantic development and the development of semantic memory. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language. Vol. I. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., Rescorla, L., Gruendel, J. & Benedict, H. (1978). Early lexicons: what do they mean? ChDev 49. 960–8.Google Scholar
Norlin, P. F. (1974). A preliminary investigation of those intralingual relational patterns which characterise the development of word meaning for preschool children. MA thesis, Northwestern University.Google Scholar
Norman, D. A. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Explorations in cognition. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Petrey, S. (1977). Word associations and the development of lexical memory. Cognition 5. 5771CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quarterman, C. J. & Riegel, K. F. (1968). Age differences in the identification of concepts of the natural language. JExpChPsych 5. 501–9.Google Scholar
Riegel, K. F. (1970 a). The language acquisition process: a reinterpretation of selected research findings. In Goulet, L. R. & Baltes, P. B. (eds), Life-span development psychology. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Riegel, K. F. (1970 b). Relational interpretation of the language acquisition process. In d'Arcais, G. B. Flores & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds), Advances in psycholinguistics. Amsterdam-London: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Riegel, K. F., Riegel, R. M., Quarterman, C. J. & Smith, H. E. (1968). An analysis of differences in word meaning and semantic structure between four education levels. HumDev 11. 92106.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., Lindsay, P. H. & Norman, D. A. (1972). A process model for long-term memory. In Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W. (eds), Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Storck, P. A. & Looft, W. R. (1973). Qualitative analysis of vocabulary responses from persons aged six to sixty plus. JEdPsych 65. 192–7.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. & Lorge, I. (1944). The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Bureau of Publications, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Wepman, J. & Lozar, B. (1973). The most frequently used words of spoken English. JPsycholingRes 2. 129–36.Google ScholarPubMed
Wolman, R. N. & Barker, E. N. (1965). A development study of word definitions. JGenPsych 107. 159–66.Google Scholar