Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:08:32.250Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The development of definitional skill*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Catherine E. Snow*
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education
*
Harvard Graduate School of Education, Larsen 7, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138, USA

Abstract

Giving good definitions requires controlling both word meaning and definitional form. Definitions from 137 second to fifth graders (i.e. aged seven to eleven) were scored to reflect conformity to the classic Aristotelian form and quality of information provided. Comparisons among children with different backgrounds indicated that school exposure to English was strongly related to per cent formal definitions given and their quality. 63 children were also tested in French, their foreign language. Performance in French was lower than in English; exposure to French at home related to the amount of information the children provided in informal definitions, not to quality of formal definitions. The results suggest that performance on definitions is most strongly affected by opportunities to practise the required form.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

I would like to express my appreciation to the Spencer Foundation, which funded collection and initial analysis of these data, to the Center for Language Education and Research, which provided further support for analysis and writing, to Herlinda Cancino, Susan Kline, and Paulina Gonzalez for their help in data collection, and to Loriana Novoa and Sara Schley for their help in statistical analysis.

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. (1970). The growth of word meaning. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Benelli, B. (1988). If it is a dog can it be an animal: the role of metalinguistic knowledge in the acquisition of linguistic superordination. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 17. 227–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benelli, B., Arcuri, L. & Marchesini, G. (1988). Cognitive and linguistic factors in the development of word definitions. Journal of Child Language 15. 619–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bierwisch, M. & Kiefer, F. (1970). Remarks on definitions in natural language. In Kiefer, F. (ed.), Studies in syntax and semantics. Dordrecht: ReidelGoogle Scholar
Binet, A. & Simon, T. (1916). The development of intelligence in children. Vineland, NJ: Publications of the Training SchoolGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Feifel, H. & Lorge, I. (1950). Qualitative differences in the vocabulary responses of children. Journal of Educational Psychology 41. 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, R. & Baillargeon, R. (1983). A review of some Piagetian concepts. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (eds), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3. Cognitive development. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
Litowitz, B. (1977). Learning to make definitions. Journal of Child Language 4. 289304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M. & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: taxonomic vs. thematic relations. Cognitive Psychology 16. 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markowitz, J. & Franz, S. (1988). The development of defining style. International Journal of Lexicography 1. 253–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. & Gildea, P. (1985). How to misread a dictionary. AILA Bulletin Pisa.Google Scholar
Snow, C., Cancino, H., De Temple, J. & Schley, S. (1990). Giving definitions: a linguistic or metalinguistic skill? In Bialystok, E. (ed.), Language processing and language awareness by bilingual children. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C., Cancino, H., Gonzalez, P. & Shriberg, E. (1989). Giving formal definitions: an oral language correlate of school literacy. In Bloome, D. (ed.), Literacy in classrooms. Norwood, NJ: AblexGoogle Scholar
Terman, L. (1916). The measurement of intelligence. Boston: Houghton-MifflinCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, R. (1985). Towards a theory of definition. Journal of Child Language 12. 181–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, R. & Olson, D. (1987). From meaning to definition: a literate bias on the structure of word meaning. In Horowitz, R. & Samuels, J. (eds), Comprehending oral and written language. Orlando: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Waxman, S. & Gelman, R. (1986). Preschoolers' use of superordinate relations in classification and language. Cognitive Development 1. 139–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Fourth edition. New York: Wilkins & WilkinsGoogle Scholar