Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:12:40.542Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Delayed acquisition of non-adjacent vocalic distributional regularities*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 March 2015

NAYELI GONZALEZ-GOMEZ*
Affiliation:
Oxford Brookes University, Department of Psychology, Oxford, England, and Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France
THIERRY NAZZI
Affiliation:
Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France, and CNRS (Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, UMR 8158), Paris, France
*
Address for correspondence: Nayeli Gonzalez-Gomez, Oxford Brookes University, Psychology Department, Gipsy Lane, Oxford, OX3 0BP, UK. tel: +44 1 865 48 36 76; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The ability to compute non-adjacent regularities is key in the acquisition of a new language. In the domain of phonology/phonotactics, sensitivity to non-adjacent regularities between consonants has been found to appear between 7 and 10 months. The present study focuses on the emergence of a posterior–anterior (PA) bias, a regularity involving two non-adjacent vowels. Experiments 1 and 2 show that a preference for PA over AP (anterior–posterior) words emerges between 10 and 13 months in French-learning infants. Control experiments show that this bias cannot be explained by adjacent or positional preferences. The present study demonstrates that infants become sensitive to non-adjacent vocalic distributional regularities between 10 and 13 months, showing the existence of a delay for the acquisition of non-adjacent vocalic regularities compared to equivalent non-adjacent consonantal regularities. These results are consistent with the CV hypothesis, according to which consonants and vowels play different roles at different linguistic levels.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This study was conducted with the support of a CONACYT grant to NGG, and ANR-13-BSH2-0004 and LABEX EFL (ANR-10-LABX-0083) grants to TN. Special thanks to the infants and their parents for their kindness and cooperation.

References

REFERENCES

Acha, J. & Perea, M. (2010). Does kaniso activate casino? Experimental Psychology [formerly Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie] 57, 245–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonatti, L. L., Pena, M., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. (2005). Linguistic constraints on statistical computations: the role of consonants and vowels in continuous speech processing. Psychological Science 16, 451–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutler, A., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Soler-Vilageliu, O. & van Ooijen, B. (2000). Constraints of vowels and consonants on lexical selection: cross-linguistic comparisons. Memory & Cognition 28, 746–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delle Luche, C., Poltrock, S., Goslin, J., New, B., Floccia, C. & Nazzi, T. (2014). Lexical access in the auditory modality: differential processing of consonants and vowels. Journal of Memory and Language 72, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farkas, I. (2009). Learning non-adjacent dependencies with a simple recurrent network. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 5507, 292–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Floccia, C., Nazzi, T., Delle Luche, C., Poltrock, S. & Goslin, J. (2014). English-learning one- to two-year-olds do not show a consonant bias in word learning. Journal of Child Language 41, 1085–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D. & Wessels, J. M. I. (1993). Phonotactic knowledge and its use in infant speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics 54, 287–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervain, J., Berent, I. & Werker, J. F. (2012). Binding at birth: the newborn brain detects identity relations and sequential position in speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24, 564–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Pena, M. & Mehler, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105, 14222–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, R. (2002). Learning non-adjacent dependencies with a simple recurrent network. Psychological Science 13, 431–6.Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Gomez, N. & Nazzi, T. (2012a). Acquisition of non-adjacent phonological regularities in the first year of life: evidence from a perceptual equivalent of the labial–coronal effect. Infancy 17, 498524.Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Gomez, N. & Nazzi, T. (2012b). Phonotactic acquisition in healthy preterm infants. Developmental Science 15, 885–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez-Gomez, N. & Nazzi, T. (2013). Effects of prior phonotactic knowledge on infant word segmentation: the case of non-adjacent dependencies. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 56, 840–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez-Gomez, N., Poltrock, S. & Nazzi, T. (2013). A ‘bat’ is easier to learn than a ‘tab’: effects of relative phonotactic frequency on infant word learning. PlosOne 8, e59601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez-Gomez, N., Tsuji, S., Hayashi, A., Mazuka, R. & Nazzi, T. (2014). The role of the input on the development of the LC bias: a crosslinguistic comparison. Cognition 132, 301–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graf Estes, K., Edwards, J. & Saffran, J. R. (2011). Phonotactic constraints on infant word learning. Infancy 16, 180–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Havy, M., Bertoncini, J. & Nazzi, T. (2011). Word learning and phonetic processing in preschool age children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108, 2543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Havy, M. & Nazzi, T. (2009). Better processing of consonantal over vocalic information in word learning at 16 months of age. Infancy 14, 439–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Havy, M., Serres, J. & Nazzi, T. (2014). A consonant/vowel asymmetry in word-form processing: eye-tracking evidence in childhood and in adulthood. Language & Speech 57, 254–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hochmann, J. R., Benavides-Varela, S., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. (2011). Consonants and vowels: different roles in early language acquisition. Developmental Science 14, 1445–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Höhle, B., Schmitz, M., Santelmann, L. & Weissenborn, J. (2006). The recognition of discontinuous verbal dependencies by German 19-month-olds: evidence for lexical and structural influences on infants’ early processing capacities. Language Learning and Development 2, 277300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Højen, A. & Nazzi, T. (in press). Vowel bias in Danish word-learning: processing biases are language-specific. Developmental Science.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Cutler, A.& Redanz, N. (1993a). Preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development 64, 675–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. W., Friederici, A., Wessels, J., Svenkerud, V. & Jusczyk, A. M. (1993b). Infants’ sensibility to the sound patterns of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language 32, 402–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A. & Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants’ sensibility to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language 33, 630–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ketrez, F. N. (2014). Harmonic cues in speech segmentation: a cross-linguistic corpus study on child-directed speech. Journal of Child Language 41, 439–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legendre, G., Barriere, I., Goyet, L. & Nazzi, T. (2010). Comprehension of infrequent subject–verb agreement forms: evidence from French-learning children. Child Development 81, 1859–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacNeilage, P. F. & Davis, B. L. (2000). The motor core of speech: a comparison of serial organization patterns in infants and languages. Child Development 71, 153–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2007). Phonological specificity of vowels and consonants in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language 57, 252–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Rao, S. B. & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283, 7780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattys, S. & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Phonotactic cues for segmenting of fluent speech by infants. Cognition 78, 91121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T. (2005). Use of phonetic specificity during the acquisition of new words: differences between consonants and vowels. Cognition 98, 1330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T., Barriere, I., Goyet, L., Kresh, S. & Legendre, G. (2011). Tracking irregular morpho-phonological dependencies in natural language: evidence from the acquisition of subject–verb agreement in French. Cognition 120, 119–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J. & Bijeljac-Babic, R. (2009a). Early emergence of a perceptual LC bias. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 126, 1440–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Floccia, C., Moquet, B. & Butler, J. (2009b). Bias for consonantal over vocalic information in French- and English-learning 30-month-olds: crosslinguistic evidence in early word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102, 522–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nespor, M., Pena, M. & Mehler, J. (2003). On the different roles of vowels and consonants in speech processing and language acquisition. Lingue e Linguaggio 2, 221–47.Google Scholar
New, B., Araújo, V. & Nazzi, T. (2008). Differential processing of consonants and vowels in lexical access through reading. Psychological Science 19, 1223–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
New, B. & Nazzi, T. (2014). The time course of consonant and vowel processing during word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29, 147–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L. & Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE. L'Année Psychologique 101, 447–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochet-Capellan, A. & Schwartz, J. (2005). Contraintes motrices et effet labial–coronal: stabilité de bisyllabes LC et CL répétés de manière accélérée. Actes des Rencontres Jeunes Chercheurs en Parole.Google Scholar
Rose, S. & Walker, R. (2011). Harmony systems. In Goldsmith, J., Riggle, J. & Yu, A. C. L. (eds), The handbook of phonological theory (2nd ed.) (pp. 240290). Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryding, K. (2005). A reference grammar of modern standard Arabic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santelmann, L. & Jusczyk, P. (1998). Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: evidence for limitations in processing space. Cognition 69, 105–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastián-Gallés, N. & Bosch, L. (2002). Building phonotactic knowledge in bilinguals: role of early exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28, 974–89.Google ScholarPubMed
Soderstrom, M., White, K. S., Conwell, E. & Morgan, J. L. (2007). Receptive grammatical knowledge of familiar content words and inflection in 16-month-olds. Infancy 12, 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toro, J., M. Nespor, M., Mehler, J. & Bonatti, L. L. (2008). Finding words and rules in a speech stream: functional differences between vowels and consonants. Psychological Science 19, 137–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallée, N., Rousset, I. & Boë, L. J. (2001). Des lexiques aux syllabes des langues du monde: typologies, tendances et organisations structurelles. Linx 45, 3750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Kampen, A., Parmaksiz, G., van De Vijver, R. & Höhle, B. (2008). Metrical and statistical cues for word segmentation: vowel harmony and word stress as cues to word boundaries by 6- and 9-month-old Turkish learners. In Gavarró, A. & Freitas, M. J. (Eds.), Language acquisition and development, 313–24. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar