Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T06:12:04.511Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's acquisition of nouns and verbs in Italian: contrasting the roles of frequency and positional salience in maternal language*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 February 2014

EMIDDIA LONGOBARDI*
Affiliation:
Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, University Sapienza of Rome, Italy
CLELIA ROSSI-ARNAUD
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University Sapienza of Rome, Italy
PIETRO SPATARO
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University Sapienza of Rome, Italy
DIANE L. PUTNICK
Affiliation:
Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA
MARC H. BORNSTEIN
Affiliation:
Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Emiddia Longobardi, Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, University Sapienza of Rome, Italy, Via dei Marsi 78, 00185, Rome. tel: +39 06 49917908; fax: +39 06 49917910; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Because of its structural characteristics, specifically the prevalence of verb types in infant-directed speech and frequent pronoun-dropping, the Italian language offers an attractive opportunity to investigate the predictive effects of input frequency and positional salience on children's acquisition of nouns and verbs. We examined this issue in a sample of twenty-six mother–child dyads whose spontaneous conversations were recorded, transcribed, and coded at 1;4 and 1;8. The percentages of nouns occurring in the final position of maternal utterances at 1;4 predicted children's production of noun types at 1;8. For verbs, children's growth rates were positively predicted by the percentages of input verbs occurring in utterance-initial position, but negatively predicted by the percentages of verbs located in the final position of maternal utterances at 1;4. These findings clearly illustrate that the effects of positional salience vary across lexical categories.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, NICHD. The authors would like to thank Anna Thornton for helpful discussions about linguistic issues.

References

REFERENCES

Aslin, R. N., Woodward, J., LaMendola, N. & Bever, T. G. (1996). Models of word segmentation in fluent maternal speech to infants. In Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K. (eds), Signal to syntax: bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition, 177–134. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Au, T. K., Dapretto, M. & Song, Y. K. (1994). Input vs constraints: early word acquisition in Korean and English. Journal of Memory & Language 33, 567–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babelot, G. & Marcos, H. (1999). Comprehension of directives in young children: influence of social situation and linguistic form. First Language 56, 165–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bassano, D. (2000). Early development of nouns and verbs in French: exploring the interface between lexicon and grammar. Journal of Child Language 27, 521–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E. & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology 40, 293310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, ,… & Hartung, J. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 21, 85123.Google Scholar
Bergelson, E. & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109, 3253–8.Google Scholar
Blackwell, A. A. (2005). Acquiring the English adjective lexicon: relationship with input properties and adjectival semantic typology. Journal of Child Language 32, 535–62.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Tackeff, J. & Lahey, M. (1984). Learning to in complement constructions. Journal of Child Language 11, 391406.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Maital, S., Painter, K., Sung-Yun, P., Pascual, L., … & Vyt, A. (2004). Cross-linguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean and American English. Child Development 75, 1115–39.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., De Houwer, A. & Putnick, D. L. (2013). Relations between lexical comprehension and production: Longitudinal evidence from bilingual infants and toddlers. Unpublished manuscript, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O. & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: a multivariate model. Journal of Child Language 25, 367–93.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Painter, K. M. & Genevro, J. L. (2000). Child language with mother and with stranger at home and in the laboratory: a methodological study. Journal of Child Language 27, 407–20.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tal, J., Rahn, C., Galperín, C. Z., Pêcheux, M.-G., Lamour, M., … & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1992). Functional analysis of the contents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology 28, 593603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Haynes, O. M. (1999). First words in the second year: continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Infant Behavior and Development 22, 6585.Google Scholar
Brent, M. R. & Siskind, J. M. (2001). The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition 81, 3344.Google Scholar
Brown, P. (1998). Children's first verbs in Tzeltal: evidence from the early verb category. Linguistics 36, 713–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L. & Longobardi, E. (1995). Nature and stability of individual differences in early lexical development of Italian-speaking children. First Language 15, 203–18.Google Scholar
Camaioni, L. & Longobardi, E. (2001). Noun versus verb emphasis in Italian mother-to-child speech. Journal of Child Language 28, 773–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caramazza, A. & Hillis, A. E. (1991). Lexical organization of nouns and verbs in the brain. Nature 349, 788–90.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, J., Fenson, L., Sanderl, L. & Weir, J. (1995). A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development 10, 159–99.Google Scholar
Choi, S. & Gopnik, A. (1995). Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: a cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 22, 497529.Google Scholar
Cornell, E. H. & Bergstrom, L. I. (1983). Serial-position effects in infants’ recognition memory. Memory & Cognition 11, 494–9.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. & Tranel, D. (1993). Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 90, 4957–60.Google Scholar
Daniele, A., Giustolisi, L., Silveri, M. C., Colosimo, C. & Gainotti, G. (1994). Evidence for a possible neuroanatomical basis for lexical processing of nouns and verbs. Neuropsychologia 32, 1325–41.Google Scholar
D'Odorico, L., Carubbi, S., Salerni, N. & Calvo, V. (2001). Vocabulary development in Italian children: a longitudinal evaluation of quantitative and qualitative aspects. Journal of Child Language 28, 351–72.Google Scholar
D'Odorico, L. & Fasolo, M. (2007). Nouns and verbs in the vocabulary acquisition of Italian children. Journal of Child Language 34, 891907.Google Scholar
D'Odorico, L. & Jacob, V. (2006). Prosodic and lexical aspects of maternal linguistic input to late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 41, 293311.Google Scholar
D'Odorico, L., Salerni, N., Cassibba, R. & Jacob, V. (1999). Stability and change of maternal speech to infants from 7 to 21 months of age: a longitudinal study of its influence on early stages of language acquisition. First Language 19, 313–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. & Gillis, S. (1998). Dutch child language: an overview. In Gillis, S. & De Houwer, A. (eds), The acquisition of Dutch, 1100. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J. & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59, 1173.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27, 209–21.Google Scholar
Fisher, C. & Tokura, H. (1996). Prosody in speech to infants: direct and indirect acoustic cues to syntactic structure. In Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K. (eds), Signal to syntax: bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition, 343363. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M. & Gobet, F. (2009). Simulating the referential properties of Dutch, German, and English root infinitives in MOSAIC. Language Learning and Development 5, 129.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Kuczaj, S. A. (ed.), Language development: Vol. 2. Language, thought, and culture, 301334. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. (2006). Why verbs are hard to learn. In Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (eds), Action meets word: how children learn verbs, 544564. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relational relativity and early word learning. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. (eds), Language acquisition and conceptual development, 215256. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. & Boroditsky, L. (2009). Early acquisition of nouns and verbs: evidence from Navajo. In Gathercole, V. (ed.), Routes to language: studies in honor of Melissa Bowerman, 536. New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A. & Trueswell, J. (2005). Hard words. Language Learning and Development 1, 2364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldfield, B. (1993). Nouns bias in maternal speech to one-year-olds. Journal of Child Language 20, 85100.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. & Alioto, A. (1995). Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: implications for language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 22, 703–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R. M. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2008). How toddlers begin to learn verbs. Trends in Cognitive Science 12, 397403.Google Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development 73, 418–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1992). How should frequency in input be measured? First Language 12, 233–44.Google Scholar
Hopper, P. J. & Thompson, S. A. (1984). The discourse basis for lexical categories in universal grammar. Language 60, 703–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.Google Scholar
Imai, M., Haryu, E. & Okada, H. (2005). Mapping novel nouns and verbs onto dynamic action events: are verb meanings easier to learn than noun meanings for Japanese children? Child Development 76, 340–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imai, M., Li, L., Haryu, E., Okada, H., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. & Shigematsu, J. (2008). Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese-, English-, and Japanese-speaking children. Child Development 79, 9791000.Google Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Bates, E. & Gutierrez-Clellen, V. (1993). Early lexical development in Spanish-speaking infants and toddlers. Journal of Child Language 20, 523–49.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology 29, 123.Google Scholar
Kemler, N. D. G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Jusczyk, P. W. & Cassidy, K. W. (1989). How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. Journal of Child Language 16, 5568.Google Scholar
Kim, M., McGregor, K. K. & Thompson, C. K. (2000). Early lexical development in English- and Korean-speaking children: language-general and language-specific patterns. Journal of Child Language 27, 224–54.Google Scholar
Laiacona, M. & Caramazza, A. (2004). The noun/verb dissociation in language production: varieties of causes. Cognitive Neuropsychology 21, 103–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, J. (1968). Introduction to theoretical linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Maital, S. L., Dromi, E., Sagi, A. & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). The Hebrew Communicative Development Inventory: language specific properties and cross-linguistic generalizations. Journal of Child Language 27, 4367.Google Scholar
Markman, E. M. (1987). How children constrain the possible meanings of words. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization, 255287. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Miceli, G., Silveri, M. C., Villa, G. & Caramazza, A. (1984). On the basis for the agrammatic's difficulty in producing main verbs. Cortex 20, 207–20.Google Scholar
Mintz, T. H., Newport, E. L. & Bever, T. G. (2002). The distributional structure of grammatical categories in speech to young children. Cognitive Science 26, 393424.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. R. & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. Journal of Child Language 25, 95120.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. A. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. A. & Ferguson, C. E. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition, 109149. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
O'Grady, W. (1997). Syntactic Development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ogura, T., Dale, P. S., Yamashita, Y., Murase, T. & Mahieu, A. (2006). The use of nouns and verbs by Japanese children and their caregivers in book-reading and toy-playing contexts. Journal of Child Language 33, 129.Google Scholar
Papailiou, C. & Rescorla, L. (2011). Vocabulary development in Greek children: a cross-linguistic comparison using the Language Development Survey. Journal of Child Language 38, 861–87.Google Scholar
Richards, B. J. & Robinson, P. (1993). Environmental correlates of child copula verb growth. Journal of Child Language 20, 343–62.Google Scholar
Robins, R. (1952). Noun and verb in universal grammar. Language 28, 289–98.Google Scholar
Robins, R. (1979). A short history of linguistics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Salerni, N., Assanelli, A., D'Odorico, L. & Rossi, G. (2007). Qualitative aspects of productive vocabulary at the 200- and 500-word stages: a comparison between spontaneous speech and parental report data. First Language 27, 7587.Google Scholar
Seidl, A. & Johnson, E. (2006). Infant word segmentation revisited: edge alignment facilitates target extraction. Developmental Science 9, 565–73.Google Scholar
Shady, M. E. & Gerken, L. A. (1999). Grammatical and caregiver cues in early sentence comprehension. Journal of Child Language 26, 113.Google Scholar
Shapiro, K. A., Moo, L. R. & Caramazza, A. (2006). Cortical signatures of noun and verb production. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103, 1644–9.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development, 175208. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, I. & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language 35, 869902.Google Scholar
Sorensen, J. M., Cooper, W. E. & Paccia, J. M. (1978). Speech timing of grammatical categories. Cognition 6, 135–53.Google Scholar
Stoll, S., Bickel, B., Lieven, E., Paudyal, N. P., Banjade, G., Bhatta, , … & Ray, N. K. (2012). Nouns and verbs in Chintang: children's usage and surrounding adult speech. Journal of Child Language 39, 284321.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Baumwell, L. & Damast, A. M. (1996). Responsive parenting in the second year: specific influences on children's language and play. Early Development and Parenting 5, 173–83.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Cyphers, L., Toda, S. & Ogino, M. (1992). Language and play at one year: A comparison of toddlers and mothers in the United States and Japan. International Journal of Behavioral Development 15, 1942.Google Scholar
Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: evidence from Mandarin speakers’ early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology 32, 492504.Google Scholar
Tardif, T., Gelman, S. A. & Xu, F. (1999). Putting the ‘noun bias’ in context: a comparison of English and Mandarin. Child Development 70, 620–35.Google Scholar
Tardif, T., Shatz, M. & Naigles, L. R. (1997). Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs: a comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language 24, 535–65.Google Scholar
Tranel, D., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. R. (2001). A neural basis for the retrieval of words for actions. Cognitive Neuropsychology 18, 655–74.Google Scholar
Vibbert, M. & Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Specific associations between domains of mother–child interaction and toddler referential language and pretense play. Infant Behavior and Development 12, 163–84.Google Scholar
Waxman, S., Fu, X., Arunachalam, S., Leddon, E., Geraghty, K. & Song, H. (2013). Are nouns learned before verbs? Infants provide insight into a long-standing debate. Child Development Perspectives 7, 155–9.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A. & Waxman, S. R. (2010). What's in the input? Frequent frames in child-directed speech offer distributional cues to grammatical categories in Spanish and English. Journal of Child Language 37, 1089–108.Google Scholar