Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:23:34.643Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Before the beginning: the development of tools of the trade

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 July 2014

DAN I. SLOBIN*
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley

Abstract

Before the beginning of the Journal of Child Language in 1974 three technological innovations had set the stage for a fundamental expansion and revision of our understanding of the processes of child language acquisition. Portable audio and video recorders changed both the quantity and quality of data. Computers made it possible to store and search large corpora, to rapidly carry out statistical analyses of the distribution of linguistic forms and their changes over time, and to simulate models of acquisition. This essay is a personal and historical overview of the unexpected consequences of those technological innovations for the study of child language. In looking to the future, another tool, linguistic typology, is essential for building models of language acquisition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Behrens, H. (ed.) (2007). Corpora in language acquisition research: history, methods, perspectives. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bar Adon, A. & Leopold, W. F. (eds) (1971). Child language: a book of readings. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single-word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa (1973). Early syntactic development: a cross-linguistic study, with special reference to Finnish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. & Brown, P. (eds) (2008). Crosslinguistic perspectives on argument structure: implications for learnability. New York/London: Erlbaum (Taylor & Francis Group).Google Scholar
Bowerman, M., de León, L. & Choi, S. (1995). Verbs, particles, and spatial semantics: learning to talk about spatial actions in typologically different languages. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Child Language Research Forum, 101–10. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1963). The ontogeny of English phrase structure. Language 39, 113.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. & Fraser, C. (1963). The acquisition of syntax. In Cofer, C. N. & Musgrave, B. S. (eds), Verbal behavior and learning, 158–97. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). Foreword. In Clark, E. V. (ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Child Language Research Forum, xixii. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (1974). Editorial. Journal of Child Language 1, iii.Google Scholar
Elman, J., Bates, E., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: a connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1969). The development of sociolinguistics. Items 32, 6176.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D., I. (eds) (1973). Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Feldman, H. (1975). The creation of a communication system: a study of deaf children of hearing parents. Sign Language Studies 8, 225–34.Google Scholar
Kessel, F. (ed.) (1988). The development of language and language researchers: essays in honor of Roger Brown. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Klima, E. S. & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lamb, S. (1961). The digital computer as an aid in linguistics. Language 37, 382412.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (2013). A history of psycholinguistics: the pre-Chomskyan era. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, Y. (1997). Autonomous linguistic systems in the language of young children. Journal of Child Language 24, 651–71.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2013). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk – electronic edition. Online: <http://childes.psy.cmu.edu/>.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Snow, C. E. (1985). The Child Language Data Exchange System. Journal of Child Language 12, 271–95.Google Scholar
Miller, W. & Ervin, S. M. (1964). The development of grammar in child language. In Bellugi, U. & Brown, R. (eds), The acquisition of language, 935. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 29(1).Google Scholar
Newport, E. L. & Meier, R. P. (1985). The acquisition of American Sign Language. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 1: the data, 881938. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievements and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child Language 5, 115.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. (1979). Transcription as theory. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (eds), Developmental pragmatics, 4372. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ratner, N. & Bruner, J. S. (1978). Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language. Journal of Child Language 5, 391401.Google Scholar
Rispoli, M. (1999). Rethinking innateness: review article and discussion. Journal of Child Language 26, 217–60.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (eds), Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Vol. 2: psychological and biological models, 216–71. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D., I. (ed.) (1967). A field manual for cross-cultural study of the acquisition of communicative competence. Berkeley, CA: Language-Behavior Research Laboratory.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1971). Developmental psycholinguistics. In Dingwall, W. O. (ed.), A survey of linguistic science, 298411. College Park, MD: University of Maryland Linguistics Program.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development, 175208. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (ed.) (1997a). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: Vol. 4. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (ed.) (1997b). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: Vol. 5. Expanding the contexts. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D., I. (ed.) (1985a). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 1: the data. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D., I. (ed.). (1985b). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 2: theoretical issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1988). From the Garden of Eden to the Tower of Babel. In Kessel, F. (ed.), The development of language and language researchers: essays in honor of Roger Brown, 922. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D., I. (ed.) (1992). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: Vol. 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. & Bowerman, M. (2007). Interfaces between linguistic typology and child language research. Linguistic Typology 11, 213–26.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I., Bowerman, M., Brown, P., Eisenbeiss, S. & Narasimhan, B. (2011). Putting things in places: developmental consequences of linguistic typology. In Bohnemeyer, J. & Pederson, E. (eds), Event representation in language and cognition, 134–65. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1979). Conversations with children. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition: studies in first language development, 363–76. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar