Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T09:52:03.179Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Baby sign but not spontaneous gesture predicts later vocabulary in children with Down Syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 June 2015

ŞEYDA ÖZÇALIŞKAN*
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
LAUREN B. ADAMSON
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
NEVENA DIMITROVA
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
JHONELLE BAILEY
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
LAUREN SCHMUCK
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
*
Address for correspondence: Şeyda Özçalışkan, Georgia State University, Department of Psychology, PO Box 5010, Atlanta, GA 30302-5010. tel: (404) 413-6282; fax: (404) 413-6207; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Early spontaneous gesture, specifically deictic gesture, predicts subsequent vocabulary development in typically developing (TD) children. Here, we ask whether deictic gesture plays a similar role in predicting later vocabulary size in children with Down Syndrome (DS), who have been shown to have difficulties in speech production, but strengths in spontaneous gesture and baby sign use. We compared the gestures and baby signs produced by twenty-three children with DS (Mage = 2;6) and twenty-three TD children (Mage = 1;6), in relation to their expressive spoken vocabulary size one year later. Children with DS showed significant deficits in gesture production, particularly for deictic gestures, but strengths in baby sign production, compared to their typically developing peers. More importantly, it was the baby signs produced by children with DS, but not deictic gestures, that predicted their spoken vocabulary size one year later. Our results further highlight the important role baby signs can play in language development in children with developmental disorders.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abrahamsen, A., Cavallo, M. M. & McCluer, J. A. (1985). Is the sign advantage a robust phenomenon? From gesture to language in two modalities. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 31, 177209.Google Scholar
Acredolo, L. P. & Goodwyn, S. W. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in in normal infants. Child Development 59, 450–66.Google Scholar
Acredolo, L. P. & Goodwyn, S. W. (1996). Baby signs. Chicago, IL: Contemporary Books.Google Scholar
Adamson, L. B. & Bakeman, R. (2006). The development of displaced speech in early mother–child conversations. Child Development 77, 186200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R. & Deckner, D. F. (2004). The development of symbol-infused joint engagement. Child Development 75, 1171–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Deckner, D. F. & Romski, M. A. (2009). Joint engagement and the emergence of language in children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39, 8496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adamson, L. B., Deckner, D. F. & Bakeman, R. (2010). Early interests and joint engagement in typical development, autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Other Developmental Disorders 40, 665–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M. & Cicchetti, D. (1987). An organizational approach to symbolic development in children with Down syndrome. In Cicchetti, D. and Beeghly, M. (eds), Atypical symbolic development, 530. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Broadley, I., MacDonald, J. & Buckley, S. J. (1995). Working memory in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice 3(1), 38.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Vicari, S., Longobardi, E., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C. & Stella, G. (1998). Gestures and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41, 1125–35.Google Scholar
Clibbens, J. (2001). Signing and lexical development in children with Down Syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice 7, 101–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(2), 6670.Google Scholar
Dimitrova, N., Özçalışkan, S. & Adamson, L. B. (2014). Parents' translations of child gesture facilitate word learning in children with autism, Down syndrome and typical development. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S. & Reilly, J. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: Users’ guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Press.Google Scholar
Franco, F. & Wishart, J. (1995). Use of pointing and other gestures by young children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation 100, 160–82.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E. & Iverson, J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science 10(6), 778–85.Google Scholar
Goodwyn, S., Acredolo, L. & Brown, C. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24, 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9, 2343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E. & Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development 14, 5775.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16(5), 367371.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Longobardi, E. & Caselli, M. C. (2003). Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down's syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 38, 179–97.Google Scholar
Jarrold, C. & Baddeley, A. (2001). Short-term memory in Down syndrome: applying the working memory model. Down Syndrome Research and Practice 7(1), 1724.Google Scholar
Johnston, J., Durieux-Smith, A. & Bloom, K. (2005). Teaching gestural signs to infants to advance child development: a review of the evidence. First Language 25, 235–51.Google Scholar
Kirk, E., Howlett, N., Pine, K. J. & Fletcher, B. (2013). To sign or not to sign? The impact of encouraging infants to gesture on infant language and maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development 84(2), 574–90.Google Scholar
Kouri, T. (1989). How manual sign acquisition relates to the development of spoken language: a case study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 20, 5062.Google Scholar
Launonen, K. (1996). Enhancing communication skills of children with Down syndrome: early use of manual signs. In von Tetzchner, S. & Jensen, M. H. (eds), Augmentative and alternative communication: European perspectives, 213–31. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: what gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F. (1992). Development of speech and language in children with Down syndrome. In Lott, I. T. & McLoy, E. E. (eds), Clinical care for persons with Down Syndrome, 3950. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F. (1999). Profiles of language development in children with Down syndrome. In Miller, J., Leddy, M. & Leavitt, L. (eds), Improving the communication of people with Down Syndrome, 1140. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Kasari, C., Sigman, M. & Ruskin, E. (1995). Nonverbal communication and early language acquisition in children with Down syndrome and in normally developing children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 157–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Kasari, C. & Yirmiya, N. (1989). Nonverbal communication skills in Down syndrome children. Child Development 59, 235–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005a). Do parents lead their children by the hand? Journal of Child Language 32(3), 481505.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005b). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition 96(3), B10113.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In Stam, G. & Ishino, M. (eds), Integrating gestures: the interdisciplinary nature of gesture, 163–74. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Paling, J. (2007). Critical review: Does baby sign language have a positive affect on language development of typically developing, hearing infants of hearing parents? Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Rowe, M., Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: gesture's role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language 28(2), 182–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer Harris, N., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W. & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down Syndromes. Developmental Neuropsychology 13, 345–70.Google Scholar
Stefanini, S., Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V. (2007). Spoken and gestural production in a naming task by young children with Down syndrome. Brain and Language 101(3), 208–21.Google Scholar
Stefanini, S., Recchia, M. & Caselli, M. C. (2008). The relationship between spontaneous gesture production and spoken lexical ability in children with Down syndrome in a naming task. Gesture 8(2), 197218.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Atypical language development: autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. In Hoff, E. & Shatz, M. (eds), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 432453). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Noin, I., Baumberger, T., Anderson, M. & Chadwick-Denis, A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down Syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20, 122.Google Scholar
Walker, M. (1977). Teaching sign language to deaf mentally handicapped adults (a practical account and an experimental evaluation), in IMS Conference Proceedings 3, Language and the mentally handicapped (pp. 3–25). Kidderminster: British Institute of Mental Handicap.Google Scholar
Wetherby, A. M., Yonclas, D. G. & Bryan, A. A. (1989). Communicative profiles of preschool children with handicaps: implications for early identification. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54, 148–58.Google Scholar
Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test, 1st ed.Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Woll, B. & Grove, N. (1996). On language deficits and modality in children with Down syndrome: a case study. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 1, 271–8.Google Scholar
Zampini, L. (2008). Produzione gestuale e pre-verbale a 24 mesi in bambini con sindrome di Down e bambini parlatori tardivi [Gestural and pre-verbal communication in 24-month- old children with Down syndrome and late-talking children]. Psicologia Clinica Dello Sviluppo 3, 511–27.Google Scholar
Zampini, L. & D'Odorico, L. (2011). Gesture production and language development: a longitudinal study of children with Down syndrome. Gesture 11(2), 174–93.Google Scholar