Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T23:00:18.723Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The circadian rhythm of rumination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 March 2009

J. G. Gordon
Affiliation:
The Rowett Research Institute, Bucksburn, Aberdeen, Scotland, AB2 9 SB
I. K. McAllister
Affiliation:
The Rowett Research Institute, Bucksburn, Aberdeen, Scotland, AB2 9 SB

Summary

Three experiments were carried out with adult sheep in order to estimate the effect of light and darkness in determining the incidence pattern of rumination.

In the first experiment the jaw movements of four sheep were recorded when they were housed in cages in a single room. Continuous records were made before and after black-out was applied to the room in order to investigate the effects of times of lighting and of feeding. The results showed that under these conditions the pattern of ruminating behaviour changed with the lighting system used and that the different feeding times had no effect upon rumination.

The first experiment was repeated with four sheep. Equal 12 h light or dark periods running from 10.00 and 22.00 h were applied and also days of continuous light or darkness. Food was given as equal meals at the beginning of each 12 h period. The results confirmed those of the first experiment and showed that rumination was greatest during the second half of each 12 h period of darkness. With permanent light or darkness rumination was least in the period from 10.00 to 16.00 h.

An attempt was made in the third experiment to impose a 16 h cycle on four sheep. The results showed no adaptation to a 16 h ‘day’. Instead, behaviour was circadian and probably based on cues external to the experimental room.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1970

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alexander, A. S. (1882). The treatment of Border Leicester ewes and lambs. Trans. Highld agric. Soc. Scotl. (4th Ser.), 14, 140–70.Google Scholar
Aschoff, J. (ed.) (1965). Circadian Clocks. Proc. Feldafing Summer School, 1964. Amsterdam: North Holland Publ. Co.Google Scholar
Bell, F. R. (1958). The electroencephalogram of conscious goats and its association with the ruminant state. J. Physiol, Lond. 143, 46P47P.Google Scholar
Bell, F. R. & Lawn, A. M. (1957). The pattern of rumination behaviour in housed goats. Br. J. Anim. Behav. 5, 8589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, F. G. (1904). Studies in body temperature. 1. Influence of the inversion of the daily routine; the temperature of night workers. Am. J. Physiol. 11, 145–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bligh, J. & Robinson, S. G. (1965). Radiotelemetry in a veterinary research project. Med. biol. Illust. 15, 9499.Google Scholar
Bryant, M. P. & Robinson, I. M. (1961). An improved nonselective culture medium for ruminal bacteria and its use in determining diurnal variation in numbers of bacteria in the rumen. J. Dairy Sci. 44, 1446–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cloudsley-Thompson, J. L. (1953). Diurnal rhythms in animals. In Science News, no. 28, pp. 7698. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Cloudsley-Thompson, J. L. (1961). Rhythmic activity in animal physiology and behaviour. In Theoretical and Experimental Biology, vol. I. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. P. (1950). Day length and head formation in the ryegrasses. J. Br. Orassld Soc. 5, 105–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dryden, J. (C 1672). Marriage à la mode.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, C. L. (ed.) (1949). Starling's Principles of Human Physiology, 10th ed.London: Churchill.Google Scholar
Faarvang, H. J. & Lauritsen, O. S. (1965). Circadian variations in sensitivity to glucocorticoid evaluated by urinary trypsin inhibitor excretion. Proc. Soc. exp. Biol. Med. 120, 338–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Facto, L. A., Diaz, F., Hays, V. W. & Catron, D. V. (1959). Time lapse cinematography as a method of studying animal behaviour and ration preferences.J. Anim. Sci. 18, 1488.Google Scholar
Gordon, J. G. (1955). Rumination in the sheep. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Aberdeen.Google Scholar
Gordon, J. G. (1958a). The act of rumination. J. agric. Sci., Camb. 50, 3442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, J. G. (1958b). The effect of time of feeding upon rumination. J. agric. Sci., Camb. 51, 8183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hancock, J. (1954). Studies in grazing behaviour of dairy cattle: bloat in relation to grazing behaviour. J. agric. Sci., Camb. 45, 8095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardison, W. A., Fisher, H. L., Ghat, G. C. & Thompson, N. R. (1956). Some observations on the behaviour of grazing lactating cows. J. Dairy Sci. 39, 1735–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harker, J. E. (1964). The Physiology of Diurnal Rhythms. Cambridge monographs on experimental biology, no. 13. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harker, K. W., Taylor, J. L. & Rollinson, D. H. L. (1954). Studies on the habits of Zebu cattle. 1. Preliminary observations on grazing habits. J. agric. Sci., Camb. 44, 193–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, D. S., Bennet, J. W., Hutchinson, J. C. D. & Woodzioka-Tomaszewska, M. (1963). Reversed photoperiodic seasons and wool growth. Nature, Lond. 198, 310–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, G. P. & Reid, D. (1951). Studies on the behaviour of cattle and sheep in relation to the utilization of grass. J. agric. Sci., Camb. 41, 350–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langlands, J. P. (1965). Diurnal variation in the diet selected by free-grazing sheep. Nature, Lond. 207, 666–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leese, A. S. (1927). A Treatise on the One-Humped Camel. Stamford: Haynes and Son.Google Scholar
Lutherer, L. O., Folk, G. E. Jr. & Essler, W. O. (1962). Daily activity pattern of reindeer in Arctic continuous light. Am. Zool. 2, 536.Google Scholar
Mech, L. D., Heezen, K. L. & Siniff, D. B. (1966). Onset and cessation of activity in cottontail rabbits and snowshoe hares in relation to sunset and sunrise. Anim. Behav. 14, 410–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills, J. N. (1966). Human circadian rhythms. Physiol. Rev. 46, 128–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, C. G., Lincoln, D. W. & Charles, D. R. (1965). The influence of constant and fluctuating environmental temperatures on time of oviposition under continuous lighting. Br. Poult. Sci. 6, 9395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearce, G. R. (1965). Rumination in sheep. 2. The circadian pattern of rumination. Aust. J. agric. Res. 16, 635–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purser, D. B. (1961). A diurnal cycle for holotrich protozoa of the rumen. Nature, Lond. 190, 831–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purser, D. B. & Moir, R. J. (1959). Ruminal flora studies in the sheep. 9. The effect of pH on the ciliate population of the rumen in vivo. Aust. J. agric. Res. 10, 555–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rakes, A. H., Hardison, W. A., Albert, J., Moore, W. E. C. & Graf, G. C. (1957). Response of growing dairy heifers to frequency of feeding. J. Dairy Sci. 40, 1621–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruckebusch, Y. (1963). Etude polygraphique et comportementale de l'evolution post-natale du sommeil chez l'agneau. Archs ital. Biol. 101, 111–32.Google Scholar
Ruckebusch, Y. & Laplace, J.-P. (1967). Recherches sur le comportement alimentaire chez les ruminants.5. Influence de l'alternance lumière-obscurité. Revue Méd. vét. 118, 587–95.Google Scholar
Scott, J. P. (1958). Animal Behaviour. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Tribe, D. E. (1948). The grazing habits of sheep. Scott. Agric. 27, 161–64.Google Scholar
Wilson, W. O., Woodard, A. E. & Abplanalp, H. (1964). Exogenous regulation of oviposition in chicken. Poult. Sci. 43, 1187–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar