Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:42:25.411Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What should we do if we were wrong and Alzheimer was right?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2013

John C. S. Breitner*
Affiliation:
Canada Research Chair in Prevention of Dementia; Pfizer Chair in Dementia Research; Centre for Studies on Prevention of Alzheimer's Disease, Douglas Mental Health University Institute; and Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada Email: [email protected]

Extract

For more than 2,000 years cognitive decline and dementia were considered a part of aging, like graying of hair, wrinkling of skin, or (more recently) reduction of pulmonary capacity or glomerular filtration rate. Thus, our recent predecessors considered “senile dementia” a normal consequence of the aging process. Their confidence increased after Martin Roth and colleagues added specificity to the concept by differentiating it within the broader category of “senile psychosis” (which, consequently, became obsolete). A diagnosis of “Alzheimer's pre-senile dementia,” by contrast, was reserved for people with earlier onset of dementia.

Type
Guest Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2013 

For more than 2,000 years cognitive decline and dementia were considered a part of aging, like graying of hair, wrinkling of skin, or (more recently) reduction of pulmonary capacity or glomerular filtration rate. Thus, our recent predecessors considered “senile dementia” a normal consequence of the aging process. Their confidence increased after Martin Roth and colleagues added specificity to the concept by differentiating it within the broader category of “senile psychosis” (which, consequently, became obsolete). A diagnosis of “Alzheimer's pre-senile dementia,” by contrast, was reserved for people with earlier onset of dementia.

This distinction between pre-senile dementia (a disease) and senile dementia (a feature of aging) was gradually abandoned following observations, again by Roth with Tomlinson and colleagues (Blessed et al., Reference Blessed, Tomlinson and Roth1968) and championed by Katzman (Reference Katzman1976) and Terry (Reference Terry, Katzman, Terry and Bick1978). These men saw what others had noted earlier (Alzheimer, Reference Alzheimer1911; Newton, Reference Newton1948; Lauter and Mayer, Reference Lauter, Mayer, Müller and Ciompi1968) that the brains of patients with the two disorders appeared strikingly similar. Thus, the 1970s and 80s saw a true paradigm shift as “senile dementia of the Alzheimer type” was recognized as every bit as much a disease as the rarer pre-senile forms. Skeptics were won over by compelling demonstrations of nonagenarians and centenarians who may have been “senile” in other ways, but certainly did not have dementia. “Alzheimer's disease” (AD), as the condition came to be known generically, could provoke dementia at almost any age, but it was not a part of normal aging. The final victory for the new paradigm came a decade later with triumphs of molecular biology. The gene for the amyloid precursor protein was cloned and sequenced; various Aβ and other cleavage products (and their aggregates) were identified; and several splice and phosphorylation variants were discovered for the microtubule associated protein tau, the precursor of neurofibrillary tangles. All of these were characteristic of “Alzheimer's disease,” no matter how young or old the patient.

Over the last 30 years, this new disease paradigm brought strong efforts to investigate the causes of all types of age-related dementia. It is difficult to overstate the value of the resulting research, most of which probably would not have occurred in an environment that regarded AD as nothing more than a phenomenon of aging. And there has indeed been some progress. However, in clinical terms at least, this progress has been painfully slow. Now may be a time, therefore, to re-examine our assumptions and shared perspectives in light of important (but sometimes overlooked) epidemiological data and their implications.

Know your enemy: what is the cumulative incidence of AD dementia by age?

Surprisingly, the literature offers little information on this topic. Several articles have attempted to estimate populations’ lifetime probability of dementia onset before death (e.g. Seshadri et al., Reference Seshadri1997; Ott et al., Reference Ott, Breteler, van Harskamp, Stijnen and Hofman1998; Yu et al., Reference Yu, Saczynski and Launer2010). In each of these papers, the cumulative risk of disease is conditioned on staying alive. That is a topic of some interest, as it asks the question, “if I am a man (or woman) currently at age x, what are the chances that I will succumb to dementia before I die?” The answer obviously depends in part on one's estimate of conditional life expectancy at age x. The latter can vary widely based on factors unrelated to a person's tendency otherwise toward dementia.

By contrast, some years ago my colleagues and I approached the cumulative incidence question a bit differently. We wanted to explore the influence of APOE genotype on AD risk in a large population (Khachaturian et al., Reference Khachaturian, Corcoran, Mayer, Zandi and Breitner2004). We sought particularly to know whether the risky ɛ4 allele increased the proportion of the population susceptible to AD dementia, or whether it simply accelerated events that would otherwise occur later. Thus, we needed an estimate of cumulative incidence in the population overall as well as an understanding of whether this estimate differed by genotype.

In the unusually long-lived Cache County population, we obtained data on AD incidence across a broad range of ages. We began by asking what proportion of the population would develop AD dementia by age 100 years, assuming that they survived other, competing risks to this age (i.e. ignoring mortality and competing risks). We next explored the influence of APOE genotype on this proportion. Findings on the first question are reproduced in Figure 1(a). Apparently, only 28% of persons would remain disease-free at age 100 years. This is equivalent to saying that 72% of persons would develop AD dementia by this age. APOE genotype did indeed modify the age-specific risk of AD dementia, but its effect was on the timing of dementia onset and not on the surprisingly high estimate of lifetime risk per se.

Figure 1. (a) Estimates of disease-free survival in the Cache County population. The figure shows Kaplan-Meier empirical survival estimates (sawtooth lines) and two Weibull models (smooth lines) fitted to the data. One Weibull model, with open circles, assumes that age-specific risk of AD onset is homogeneous, i.e. that there are no subgroups with substantially different characteristic risks-by-age. The other model, shown with open triangles, incorporates a parameter corresponding to a proportion of the population that will not develop dementia at any reasonable age. This second model gives a vastly improved fit to the data (Khachaturian et al., Reference Khachaturian, Corcoran, Mayer, Zandi and Breitner2004). As a result of the admixture of ostensibly invulnerable individuals (in reality, they may only have a much later onset characteristic), the latter curve approaches an asymptotic value at the proportion of those estimated in the model as being not at risk (approximately 25%). For several reasons cited in the text, that estimate is likely to be substantially inflated. (b) The figure shows the disease-free survival (1 – cumulative incidence), as estimated from the exponential equation fitted by Brookmeyer et al. (Reference Brookmeyer, Johnson, Ziegler-Graham and Arrighi2007) to data from several well-regarded cohort studies of AD incidence. This curve appears quite similar to the Weibull models in Figure 1(a), although it makes no attempt to discern heterogeneity. Both Figures 1(a) and (b) suggest that the large majority of individuals may be expected to develop AD dementia if they survive to age 100 years. Note: Figure 1(a) reprinted with permission.

As noted above, verification of our cumulative incidence findings in other published material was difficult. We therefore turned to a well-known synthesis by Brookmeyer et al. (Reference Brookmeyer, Johnson, Ziegler-Graham and Arrighi2007) of data from several high-quality population studies of AD incidence. As part of their work, these authors published an exponential equation that provided a best-fit model for the annual incidence of AD as a function of age. The latest iteration of this equation is as follows:

{Incidence}\,(\% \,per\,yr) = 0.117e^{0.127(x - 60)}

where x is year of age after 60. Using this equation, one can also estimate cumulative incidence by calculating the proportion surviving after each year (starting with 100% at age 60), estimating and subtracting the incidence percentage for the ensuing year, and thus arriving at the proportion surviving at the end of that year. Cumulative incidence at age x can then be calculated by repeating this process iteratively up to the year x. This procedure gives results shown in Figure 1(b). Although the latter figure does not include allowance for a stratum of the population not vulnerable to AD dementia (as in Figure 1(a)), both Figures 1(a) and (b) suggest that the cumulative incidence of AD by age 100 years will be around 75%.

The size of this percentage gives me pause. For starters, it is almost certainly an underestimate for several reasons. Survey research on dementia nearly always suffers from: (1) differential response rates with under-representation of persons with dementia (Norton et al., Reference Norton, Breitner, Welsh and Wyse1994); and (2) differential occurrence of dementia among those who die in the intervals between periodic assessments (Launer and Brock, Reference Launer and Brock2004). Another source of underestimation stems from the substantial numbers of persons (especially in older strata) who have neuropathological “Alzheimer's disease” without cognitive impairment (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Wilson, Boyle, Buchman and Schneider2012). Finally, it is clear that the brains of those with clinical diagnoses of mixed or other dementias frequently include AD pathology.

I therefore suggest that, whatever their true proportion, persons who would reach 100 years without succumbing to AD dementia are a small minority. Alternately stated, development of dementia in late old age appears to be the norm, and not the exception.

Then, is “Alzheimer's disease” really a disease?

The Oxford English Dictionary describes “disease” as a condition in which the body or some of its component parts are functionally deranged or disturbed. Usually, the implication is that the functional disturbance results from an abnormality or process that is not ordinarily expected, i.e. that it is not a change that is ubiquitous for the species. Contrariwise, if the change does become ubiquitous with the passage of time, then (by common consensus) it is often considered a part of aging. Importantly, the time-dependent process of change need not proceed at the same rate in all individuals. But it must do so in measurable fashion.

AD and many other late-life diseases challenge this distinction. These illnesses share a characteristic that they evolve over a long period of biological change while remaining largely asymptomatic. Symptoms appear only when the change reaches a certain threshold of accumulated damage. Many years ago, for example, my colleagues and I showed that the probability of AD onset was approximated (in what was certainly an oversimplified model) by an Erlangian or “multiple-hit” distribution (Breitner et al., Reference Breitner, Folstein and Murphy1986), such that symptoms became evident only after accumulation of a specified number of “hits.” One might then consider whether the chronic accumulation of “hits” (Alzheimerization) is a manifestation of aging, whereas the emergence of symptoms conforms to the notion of disease.

Along with Peter Whitehouse (Whitehouse and George, Reference Whitehouse and George2008) and others, I suggest therefore that – for AD at least – the distinction between aging and disease is far from clear. I hasten to add that there can be no doubting the heavy disabilities wrought by AD dementia and the burden it places on families and, ultimately, on society. But, in terms of its origins, my “bottom line” is that, over a full human lifespan, the development of AD dementia is more the norm than an exception. The implications of this statement appear to be profound. For those like me with a primary interest in dementia prevention, if development of AD dementia is the norm rather than the exception, then probably we should not be asking why some people get AD dementia, but instead how or why some few can live a full life and escape it. Is it possible that these “escapees” manage to avoid dementia onset only by postponing it beyond human life expectancy? We will be able to test this latter idea once we have reliable indicators for the evolving “pre-symptomatic AD” process. Until then it matters not whether, without intervention, most everyone would succumb to AD dementia at some (perhaps very late) age; the objective should be to postpone it. In this way, we may co-opt a favorite expression of the lawyers (about availability of legal remedy). “AD dementia” may be inevitable for most of us, but at some point, with successful interventions, dementia delayed may become dementia denied.

References

Alzheimer, A. (1911). Über eigenartige Krankheitsfalle des späteren Alters. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 4, 356385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Boyle, P. A., Buchman, A. S. and Schneider, J. A. (2012). Relation of neuropathology to cognition in persons without cognitive impairment. Annals of Neurology, 72, 599609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. E. and Roth, M. (1968). The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. British Journal of Psychiatry, 114, 797811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breitner, J. C. S., Folstein, M. F. and Murphy, E. A. (1986). Familial aggregation in Alzheimer dementia I: a model for the age-dependent expression of an autosomal dominant gene. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 20, 3143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brookmeyer, R., Johnson, E., Ziegler-Graham, K. and Arrighi, H. M. (2007). Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers and Dementia, 3, 186191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katzman, R. (1976). Editorial: the prevalence and malignancy of Alzheimer disease. A major killer. Archives of Neurology, 33, 217218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khachaturian, A. S., Corcoran, C., Mayer, L. S., Zandi, P. P., Breitner, J. C. and Cache County Study Investigators. (2004). Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 count affects age at onset of Alzheimer disease, but not lifetime susceptibility: the Cache County Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 518524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Launer, L. J. and Brock, D. B. (2004). Population-based studies of AD: message and methods: an epidemiologic view. Statistics in Medicine, 23, 191197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lauter, H. and Mayer, J. E. (1968). Clinical and nosological concepts of senile dementia. In Müller, C. and Ciompi, L. (eds.), Senile Dementia (pp. 1327). Berne: Huber.Google Scholar
Newton, R. D. (1948). The identity of Alzheimer's disease and senile dementia and their relationship to senility. Journal of Mental Science, 94, 225249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norton, M. C., Breitner, J. C., Welsh, K. A. and Wyse, B. W. (1994). Characteristics of nonresponders in a community survey of the elderly. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 42, 12521256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ott, A., Breteler, M. M., van Harskamp, F., Stijnen, T. and Hofman, A. (1998). Incidence and risk of dementia: the Rotterdam Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 147, 574580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seshadri, S.et al. (1997). Lifetime risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease: the impact of mortality on risk estimates in the Framingham Study. Neurology, 49, 14981504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terry, R. D. (1978). Aging, senile dementia and Alzheimer's disease. In Katzman, R., Terry, R. D. and Bick, K. (eds.), Alzheimer's Disease: Senile Dementia and Related Disorders (Aging, vol. 7; 11–14). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, P. J. and George, D. (2008). The Myth of Alzheimer's. What You aren't Being Told about Today's Most Dreaded Diagnosis. New York: St Martin's Press.Google Scholar
Yu, B., Saczynski, J. S. and Launer, L. (2010). Multiple imputation for estimating the risk of developing dementia and its impact on survival. Biometrics Journal, 52, 616627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. (a) Estimates of disease-free survival in the Cache County population. The figure shows Kaplan-Meier empirical survival estimates (sawtooth lines) and two Weibull models (smooth lines) fitted to the data. One Weibull model, with open circles, assumes that age-specific risk of AD onset is homogeneous, i.e. that there are no subgroups with substantially different characteristic risks-by-age. The other model, shown with open triangles, incorporates a parameter corresponding to a proportion of the population that will not develop dementia at any reasonable age. This second model gives a vastly improved fit to the data (Khachaturian et al., 2004). As a result of the admixture of ostensibly invulnerable individuals (in reality, they may only have a much later onset characteristic), the latter curve approaches an asymptotic value at the proportion of those estimated in the model as being not at risk (approximately 25%). For several reasons cited in the text, that estimate is likely to be substantially inflated. (b) The figure shows the disease-free survival (1 – cumulative incidence), as estimated from the exponential equation fitted by Brookmeyer et al. (2007) to data from several well-regarded cohort studies of AD incidence. This curve appears quite similar to the Weibull models in Figure 1(a), although it makes no attempt to discern heterogeneity. Both Figures 1(a) and (b) suggest that the large majority of individuals may be expected to develop AD dementia if they survive to age 100 years. Note: Figure 1(a) reprinted with permission.